html5-img
1 / 60

Session 1

Methodology Research Group. Methods of explanatory analysis for psychological treatment trials workshop. Session 1 Introduction to causal inference and the analysis of treatment effects in the presence of departures from random allocation Ian White. Funded by: MRC Methodology Grant G0600555

curt
Download Presentation

Session 1

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. MethodologyResearch Group Methods of explanatory analysis for psychological treatment trials workshop Session 1 Introduction to causal inference and the analysis of treatment effects in the presence of departures from random allocation Ian White Funded by: MRC Methodology GrantG0600555 MHRN Methodology Research Group

  2. Plan of session 1 • Describe departures from random allocation • Intention-to-treat analysis, per-protocol analysis and their limitations • What do we want to estimate? • Estimation methods: principal stratification • Instrumental variables • Structural mean model • Extensions: complex departures, missing data, covariates • Small group discussion Illustrated with data from the ODIN and SoCRATES trials

  3. Get 0 Get X Get S Get E Get X Get 0 ??? ??? ? ?? ?? ? Switches Changes to non-trial treatment Parallel-group trial Recruit Randomise Standardtreatment (S) Experimental treatment (E) Get S Get E Measure outcome Measure outcome

  4. Aim of session 1 • Infer causal effect of treatment in the presence of departures from randomised intervention • Better term than “non-compliance”: includes both non-adherence and changes in prescribed treatment • Types of departure: • Switches to other trial treatment or changes to non-trial (or no) treatment • Yes / no or quantitative (e.g. attend some sessions) • Constant or time-dependent • We’ll start by considering the simplest case: all-or-nothing switches to the other trial treatment • The methods introduced here will be used in later sessions

  5. Plan of session 1 • Describe departures from random allocation • Intention-to-treat analysis, per-protocol analysis and their limitations • What do we want to estimate? • Estimation methods: principal stratification • Instrumental variables • Structural mean model • Extensions: complex departures, missing data, covariates • Small group discussion All illustrated with data from the ODIN trial

  6. Intention-To-Treat (ITT) Principle http://www.consort-statement.org/ glossary: • “A strategy for analyzing data in which all participants are included in the group to which they were assigned, whether or not they completed the intervention given to the group. • “Intention-to-treat analysis prevents bias caused by the loss of participants, which may disrupt the baseline equivalence established by random assignment and which may reflect non-adherence to the protocol.” • Now the standard analysis – and rightly so

  7. Intention-to-treat analysis • Compare groups as randomised, ignoring any departures • Answers an important pragmatic question • e.g. the public health impact of prescribing E • Disadvantage: this may be the wrong question! • may want to explore public health impact of prescribing E outside the trial, when compliance might be less • alternative pragmatic question • may want to know the effect of receiving E • explanatory question

  8. Disadvantage of ITT • “Doctor doctor, will psychotherapy cure my depression?” • “I don’t know, but I expect prescribing psychotherapy to reduce your BDI score by 5 units … • on average … • that’s on average over whether you attend or not” • Clearly, judgements about whether a patient is likely to attend, take a drug, etc., should be a part of prescribing • But we often need to know effects of attendance, the drug, etc. in themselves

  9. Per-protocol (PP) analysis • Alternative to ITT • Exclude any data collected after a departure from randomised treatment • requires careful pre-definition: what will be counted as departures? • Idea is to exclude data that doesn’t allow for the full effect of treatment • However, PP implicitly assumes that individuals with different treatment experience are comparable • rarely true • in practice there can be substantial selection bias

  10. Alternative to ITT and PP • We adopt a “causal modelling” approach that carefully considers what we want to estimate and what assumptions are needed to do so • Estimation will avoid assumptions of comparability between groups as treated • will instead be based on comparisons of randomised groups

  11. Plan of session 1 • Describe departures from random allocation • Intention-to-treat analysis, per-protocol analysis and their limitations • What do we want to estimate? • Estimation methods: principal stratification • Instrumental variables • Structural mean model • Extensions: complex departures, missing data, covariates • Small group discussion All illustrated with data from the ODIN trial

  12. What do we want to estimate? • The effect of the intervention, if everyone had received their randomised intervention? • “average causal effect”, ACE • “average treatment effect”, ATE • conceptual difficulties: • how could we make them receive their randomised intervention? • would this be ethical? • would it have other consequences? • technical difficulties: • turns out to be unidentified (unestimable) without further strong assumptions

  13. What do we want to estimate? (2) Alternatives to the average causal effect: • “Average treatment effect in the treated”, ATT • “Complier-average causal effect”, CACE • to be defined below • Note how we separate what we want to estimate from analysis methods

  14. Counterfactuals • Consider a trial of intervention E vs. control S • Define “counterfactual” or “potential” outcomes: • Yi(1) = outcome for individual i if they received intervention • Yi(0) = outcome for individual i if they received control • We can only observe one of these! • Intervention effect for individual i is Di = Yi(1) - Yi(0) • Then average causal effect of intervention is E[Di] • the average difference between outcome with intervention and outcome with control

  15. Estimation with perfect compliance • With perfect compliance, we observe • Yi(1) in everyone in the intervention arm • Yi(0) in everyone in the control arm • Randomisation means that mean outcome with intervention can be estimated by mean outcome of those who got intervention E[Yi | R=E] – E[Yi | R=S] = E[Yi(1) | R=E] – E[Yi(0) | R=S] = E[Yi(1)] – E[Yi(0)] = E[Di] • Not true with imperfect compliance! • So ITT estimates the average causal effect of intervention

  16. Estimation with imperfect compliance • Assume “all-or-nothing” compliance • everyone gets either intervention or control • In the intervention arm, we observe • Yi(1) in compliers • Yi(0) in non-compliers • In the control arm, we observe • Yi(0) in compliers • Yi(1) in “contaminators” • Need assumptions to estimate the average causal effect of intervention • A very simple assumption is • Yi(1) - Yi(0) = b • b is the (average) causal effect of intervention

  17. Estimation with imperfect compliance (2) • Continuing with “causal model” Yi(1) - Yi(0) = b • can be written as Yi = Yi(0) + b Di • Di = 1 if intervention was received, else 0 • Implies that expected difference in outcome (between randomised groups) = causal effect of intervention x expected difference in intervention receipt • E[Yi|R=E] – E[Yi|R=S] = b{E[Di|R=E] – E[Di|R=S]} • This gives the simplest causal estimator: • causal effect of intervention = expected difference in outcome / expected difference in intervention receipt

  18. But … • Angrist, Imbens and Rubin (1996) took a different perspective and showed that this estimator isn’t what it seems • To see this, consider “counterfactual treatments”: • DiE = treatment if randomised to intervention • DiS = treatment if randomised to control • both are 0/1 (received standard / intervention) • Implies 4 types of person (“compliance-types”): • DiE=1, DiS=1: always-takers • DiE=1, DiS=0: compliers • DiE=0, DiS=0: never-takers • DiE=0, DiS=1: defiers – assumed absent

  19. Introducing the complier-average causal effect • The observed data tell us nothing about the causal effects of treatment in always-takers and never-takers • In fact, our simple estimator estimates the “complier-average causal effect” (CACE) = E[Di| DiE=1, DiS=0] • This is all we can hope to estimate in RCTs!

  20. Problems with the CACE • We don’t know who is a “complier” • In practice, we may want to know what will be observed • if compliance is worse than in the trial (e.g. if rolled out in clinical practice) • if compliance is better than in the trial (e.g. because intervention is well publicised / marketed) This means we want to know the average causal effect in a different subgroup. We might assume this is the CACE – but it is an assumption

  21. Summary of things we can estimate • ITT: E[Y|R=E] – E[Y|R=S] • PP: E[Y|R=E, DE=1] – E[Y|R=S, DS=0] • ACE/ATE: E[Y(1) – Y(0)] • ATT: E[Y(1) – Y(0) | DE=1] • CACE: E[Y(1) – Y(0) | DE=1, DS=0] We are going to explore ways to estimate the CACE

  22. Plan of session 1 • Describe departures from random allocation • Intention-to-treat analysis, per-protocol analysis and their limitations • What do we want to estimate? • Estimation methods: principal stratification • Instrumental variables • Structural mean model • Extensions: complex departures, missing data, covariates • Small group discussion All illustrated with data from the ODIN trial

  23. Principal stratification • An idea of Frangakis and Rubin (1999), generalising the simple compliance-types above • Again, let • DiE = treatment if randomised to intervention • DiS = treatment if randomised to control where both could be complex (e.g. numbers of sessions of psychotherapy) • Principal strata are the levels of the pair (DiE, DiS)

  24. Using principal stratification • We should model outcomes conditional on principal strata • typically allow a different mean for each principal stratum – avoids assuming they are comparable • allow differences between randomised groups within principal strata • these parameters have a causal meaning • Of course this may not be easy, since for every individual we only know one of (DiE, DiS) so we don’t know their principal stratum

  25. Example: ODIN trial • Trial of 2 psychological interventions to reduce depression (Dowrick et al, 2000) • Randomised individuals: • 236 to the psychological interventions (E) • 128 to treatment as usual (S) • Outcome: Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) at 6 months • recorded on 317 randomised individuals

  26. ODIN trial: compliance • Of 236 individuals randomised to psychological interventions, 128 (54%) attended in full • others refused, did not attend or discontinued • Psychological interventions weren’t available to the control arm (no “contaminators”) so DS=0 for all • Only 2 principal strata: • would attend if randomised to intervention • DE=1, “compliers” • would not attend if randomised to intervention • DE=0, “never-takers”

  27. Exclusion restriction • Key assumption used to identify the CACE • In individuals for whom randomisation has no effect on treatment (e.g. in never-takers and always-takers), randomisation has no effect on outcome • Often reasonable: e.g. in a double-blind drug trial, not taking active drug is the same as not taking placebo • But not always reasonable: e.g. not attending counselling despite being invited could be different from not attending because uninvited • “I wouldn’t have gone, but I’d like to have been invited”

  28. Exclusion restriction in ODIN • In ODIN, the exclusion restriction means that randomisation has no effect on outcomes in those who would not attend if randomised to psychological intervention • But recall that we included those who discontinued as “non-attenders” • their partial attendance is very likely to have had some effect on them • the exclusion restriction would be more plausible if we defined compliance as any attendance • we’ll return to this later

  29. CACE analysis (complete cases)

  30. complier-average causal effect (CACE) randomisation balance (59*140/177) 46.7 13.22 93.316.13 exclusion restriction CACE analysis (2) Note: 66.7% compliance (118/177)ITT / 0.667 = CACE CACE = 13.32 – 16.13 = -2.81(cf ITT = 13.29 – 15.16 = -1.87)

  31. CACE equal PP equal CACE is based on the “exclusion restriction” assumption Per-protocol analysis estimates the CACE under the “random non-compliance” assumption CACE vs. PP

  32. Plan of session 1 • Describe departures from random allocation • Intention-to-treat analysis, per-protocol analysis and their limitations • What do we want to estimate? • Estimation methods: principal stratification • Instrumental variables • Structural mean model • Extensions: complex departures, missing data, covariates • Small group discussion All illustrated with data from the ODIN trial

  33. Instrumental variables (IV) • Popular in econometrics • Model: • Model of interest: Yi = a + b Di + ei • Error ei may be correlated with Di (“endogenous”) • Example in econometrics: D is years of education, Y is adult wage, e includes unobserved confounders • We can’t estimate b by ordinary linear regression • Instead, we assume error ei is independent of an 3rdinstrumental variable Ri • i.e. Ri only affects outcome through its effect on Di • or: randomisation only affects outcome through its effect on treatment actually received

  34. IV estimation • Estimation by “two-stage least squares”: model implies • E[Yi | Ri] = a + b E[Di | Ri] • so first regress Di on Ri to get E[Di | Ri] • then regress Yi on E[Di | Ri] • NB standard errors not quite correct by this method: general IV uses different standard errors • More generally, we use an estimating equation based onSi Ri (Yi – a – b Di ) = 0

  35. Instrumental variables for ODIN . ivreg bdi6 (treata=z) Instrumental variables (2SLS) regression Source | SS df MS Number of obs = 317 -------------+------------------------------ F( 1, 315) = 2.64 Model | -58.5115086 1 -58.5115086 Prob > F = 0.1049 Residual | 32532.4232 315 103.277534 R-squared = . -------------+------------------------------ Adj R-squared = . Total | 32473.9117 316 102.765543 Root MSE = 10.163 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ bdi6 | Coef. Std. Err. t P>|t| [95% Conf. Interval] -------------+---------------------------------------------------------------- treata | -2.803511 1.724143 -1.63 0.105 -6.195802 .5887801 _cons | 15.15714 .8588927 17.65 0.000 13.46725 16.84703 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ Instrumented: treata Instruments: z ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ Same estimate as before!

  36. Easy to extend to include covariates . ivreg bdi6 (treata=z) bdi0 Instrumental variables (2SLS) regression Source | SS df MS Number of obs = 317 -------------+------------------------------ F( 2, 314) = 43.26 Model | 6808.64828 2 3404.32414 Prob > F = 0.0000 Residual | 25665.2634 314 81.7365076 R-squared = 0.2097 -------------+------------------------------ Adj R-squared = 0.2046 Total | 32473.9117 316 102.765543 Root MSE = 9.0408 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ bdi6 | Coef. Std. Err. t P>|t| [95% Conf. Interval] -------------+---------------------------------------------------------------- treata | -3.428509 1.539881 -2.23 0.027 -6.458298 -.3987196 bdi0 | .5813933 .0630405 9.22 0.000 .4573581 .7054285 _cons | 2.395561 1.546673 1.55 0.122 -.6475924 5.438714 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ Instrumented: treata Instruments: bdi0 z ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ Usual gain in precision

  37. Plan of session 1 • Describe departures from random allocation • Intention-to-treat analysis, per-protocol analysis and their limitations • What do we want to estimate? • Estimation methods: principal stratification • Instrumental variables • Structural mean model • Extensions: complex departures, missing data, covariates • Small group discussion All illustrated with data from the ODIN trial

  38. Structural mean model (SMM) • Extends our simple model Yi(1) - Yi(0) = b • SMM is E[YiE - YiC | DiE, DiC, X] = b Di* • where Di* is a summary of treatment thought to have a causal effect, e.g.: • Di* = DiE – DiC: causal effect of treatment is proportional to amount of treatment • Di* = (DiE – DiC , Xi(DiE – DiC)): and X is an effect modifier • Goetghebeur and Lapp, 1997 (assumed DiC=0) • Estimation is equivalent to instrumental variables with R and R*X as instruments • in other words, we also assume that X does not modify the causal effect of treatment

  39. Summary for binary compliance • The principal stratification approach divides individuals into always-takers, compliers and never-takers • We can then identify the complier-average causal effect, provided we make the exclusion restriction assumption • This works for binary or continuous outcomes • Instrumental variables and structural mean models approaches lead to the same estimates for continuous outcomes • For binary outcomes, instrumental variables are problematic, and generalised structural mean models are needed (Vansteelandt and Goetghebeur, 2003)

  40. Plan of session 1 • Describe departures from random allocation • Intention-to-treat analysis, per-protocol analysis and their limitations • What do we want to estimate? • Estimation methods: principal stratification • Instrumental variables • Structural mean model • Extensions: complex departures, missing data, covariates • Small group discussion All illustrated with data from the ODIN trial

  41. Example with missing outcome data • Our IV analyses of ODIN used complete cases only • This is a bad idea • Follow-up rates were worse in non-attenders (55%) than in attenders (92%) • So we modify the previous analysis • We will now assume the data are “missing at random” given randomised group and attendance • e.g. among non-attenders, there is no difference on average between non-responders and responders

  42. 128 108 236 complier-average causal effect (CACE) randomisation balance (108*191/236) 103.6 87.4 191 13.22 93.316.13 exclusion restriction 16.80 CACE analysis under MAR CACE (MAR) = 13.32 – 16.80 = -3.48cf CACE (CC) = 13.32 – 16.13 = -2.81

  43. A more general approach • We can allow for missing data by using inverse probability weights • Suppose a certain group of individuals has only 50% chance of responding • give each responder in that group a weight of 2 • accounts for their non-responding fellows • In ODIN, we will consider the baseline-adjusted analysis • We will construct weights depending on baseline BDI, randomised group and attendance

  44. Constructing the weights . logistic resp6 z treata bdi0 Logistic regression Number of obs = 427 LR chi2(3) = 49.84 Prob > chi2 = 0.0000 Log likelihood = -218.70364 Pseudo R2 = 0.1023 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ resp6 | Odds Ratio Std. Err. z P>|z| [95% Conf. Interval] -------------+---------------------------------------------------------------- z | .4327186 .1102412 -3.29 0.001 .2626333 .7129535 treata | 10.1753 3.909568 6.04 0.000 4.791789 21.60713 bdi0 | .9750455 .0136551 -1.80 0.071 .9486461 1.00218 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ . predict presp (option pr assumed; Pr(resp6)) . gen wt=1/presp

  45. Examining the weights therapy, non-compliers control therapy, compliers

  46. Weighted IV analysis . ivreg bdi6 (treata=z) bdi0 [pw=wt] (sum of wgt is 4.2710e+02) Instrumental variables (2SLS) regression Number of obs = 317 F( 2, 314) = 37.28 Prob > F = 0.0000 R-squared = 0.2183 Root MSE = 9.0521 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ | Robust bdi6 | Coef. Std. Err. t P>|t| [95% Conf. Interval] -------------+---------------------------------------------------------------- treata | -3.953868 1.944846 -2.03 0.043 -7.780444 -.1272916 bdi0 | .5810663 .0680343 8.54 0.000 .4472056 .714927 _cons | 2.37602 1.554941 1.53 0.128 -.6834003 5.435441 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ Instrumented: treata Instruments: bdi0 z ------------------------------------------------------------------------------

  47. Back to the exclusion restriction • Recall that partial attenders were included as non-compliers • If instead we include them as compliers, the exclusion restriction is much more plausible • The estimated causal effect is smaller because it is an average over a wider group that includes partial compliers

  48. Summary of ODIN results

  49. Example with continuous compliance:the SoCRATES trial • SoCRATES was a multi-centre RCT designed to evaluate the effects of cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) and supportive counselling (SC) on the outcomes of an early episode of schizophrenia. • 201 participants were allocated to one of three groups: • Control: Treatment as Usual (TAU) • Treatment: TAU plus psychological intervention, either CBT + TAU or SC + TAU • The two treatment groups are combined in our analyses • Outcome: psychotic symptoms score (PANSS) at 18 months

  50. SoCRATES: ITT results

More Related