1 / 21

Nury Rodriguez Education 7202: Seminar in Applied Theory and Research II Fall 2010

Primary Language Matters: Is There a Link Between Primary Language Proficiency and English Language Learners’ Academic Achievement?. Nury Rodriguez Education 7202: Seminar in Applied Theory and Research II Fall 2010. Introduction Statement of Problem (Slide 3)

donagh
Download Presentation

Nury Rodriguez Education 7202: Seminar in Applied Theory and Research II Fall 2010

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Primary Language Matters: Is There a Link Between Primary Language Proficiency and English Language Learners’ Academic Achievement? Nury Rodriguez Education 7202: Seminar in Applied Theory and Research II Fall 2010

  2. Introduction • Statement of Problem (Slide 3) • Review of Related Literature (Slides 4-5) • Research Hypothesis (Slide 6) • Method • Participants (Slide 7) • Instruments (Slide 8) • Experimental Design (Slide 9) • Procedure (Slide 10) • Results (Slides 11-15) • Discussion (Slide 16) • Implications (Slide 17) • References (Slide 18) Table of Contents

  3. Statement of the Problem Some English language learners are failing to meet the academic standards. Meanwhile, the debate continues of whether students who are learning English as a second language should be taught using their primary language or in an English only classroom. Some schools have adopted the transitional bilingual programs to help these children, but these transitional bilingual programs provide minimal primary language intervention. However, Cummins’ (1979) developmental interdependence hypothesis asserts that the “development of competence in a second language is partially a function of the type of competence already developed in the first language at the time when intensive exposure to the second language begins” (as cited in Lenters, 2004, p. 329). This action research will attempt to find out: What are the effects of a transitional bilingual intervention on literacy development? Is there a positive correlation between students’ primary language proficiency and target language reading development?

  4. Review of Related Literature Theorists & Practitioners

  5. Review of Related Literature

  6. Statement of the Hypothesis • HR1: Implementing a transitional bilingual Education program with nine first grade English language learners with average to high Spanish language proficiency will yield greater reading progress than the nine first grade English language learners with low Spanish language proficiency, for 40 minutes five times per week over an eight-week period at PSX in Brooklyn, NY.

  7. Method Participants • Total population of 18 first grade Latino students at PSX in Brooklyn, New York • 56% or 10 students are girls • 44% or 8 students are boys • Other Participants • Parents of participating students • Teachers and Administration • Racial breakdown of PSX in Brooklyn, New York • PSX student population consists of 1,125 students with ethnic make up of 1 percent African American, 63 percent Hispanic or Latino, Asian or Native Hawaiian/Other Pacific Islander, and 7 percent White • Limited English proficient (LEP) students are 41 percent (441 students) of the student population at PSX

  8. Method: Instruments • Consent Forms Administered to: • Parent • Assistant Principal • Teacher • Likert Scale Surveys Administered to: • Students • Parents • Assessments: • Primary Language Assessment • Reading Assessments • Pretest Developmental Reading Assessment • Posttest Developmental Reading Assessment

  9. Research Design Quasi Experimental:Nonequivalent Control Group Design Two groups: Designated treatment group (X1) and control group (X2) are pretested, exposed to a treatment (X), and posttested (O). Groups are not randomly assigned. Symbolic Design: O X1 O O X2 O

  10. Procedure • This Action Research was conducted from May 2010 to • June 2010: • Consent form were distributed and collected from Assistant Principal, teacher, and students, May 2010. • Surveys were distributed to and collected from students and parents, May 2010. • Pretest administered September 2010. • Posttest administered May 2010. • Post Surveys administered June 2010.

  11. Results:Quasi Experimental:Nonequivalent Control Group Design/ Printed Language Concepts and Reading Comprehension Pretest and Posttest Scores/Group 1 & Group 2

  12. Results:Correlation of Students Primary Language Attitudes and Posttest English Reading Assessment scores.Likert Scale Post Survey Question #2:

  13. Results: Correlation of Students Primary Language Proficiency and Posttest DRA2 Reading Levels Correlation Coefficient (rxy)=0.65. The line of best fit show a fair positive correlation between students primary language proficiency and posttest English reading levels. Therefore, if primary language proficiency is high, students reading levels are high.

  14. Results: Data Analysis Quasi Experimental: Nonequivalent Control Group Design

  15. Results: Data Analysis Quasi Experimental: Nonequivalent Control Group Design The data for Group 1 indicate that after Transitional Bilingual Education, and high levels of primary language proficiency (treatments) students are at higher reading level compared to Group 2 reading levels. • 11% of the students are reading significantly below grade level. • 22% of the students are reading below grade level. • 55% of the students are reading on grade level. • 11% of the students are reading above grade level. • The data for Group 2 indicate that after Transitional Bilingual education, and low levels of primary language proficiency (treatments) students are at lower reading levels compared to Group 1 reading levels. • 33% of the students are reading significantly below grade level. • 45% of the students are below first grade level. • 22% of the students are on grade level.

  16. Discussion This action research appears to support the hypothesis that a transitional bilingual Education program with nine first grade English language learners with high to average Spanish language proficiency to some extend achieved higher reading progress compared to English language learners with lower primary language skills. There is a correlation between reading performance and primary language proficiency as demonstrated by the higher reading levels of those student with high to moderate primary language proficiency. In line with Cummins interdependence hypothesis “instruction in primary language is effective in promoting proficiency in primary language, transfer of this proficiency to the target language will occur provided there is adequate exposure to target language.” (as cited in Garcia and Baker, 2007, p. 112.) This demonstrates the use of primary language intervention and students’ native language proficiency may increase students’ reading progress in English.

  17. Implications • The results of this action research show that primary language proficiency in a bilingual setting may be a predictor for second language acquisition. However, there are other factors to consider: • Longitudinal study is needed • Larger sample size • Transitional Bilingual Program/ESL Strategies • Other factors

  18. Threats to Internal Validity Threats to Internal Validity Threats to External Validity Ecological Validity Generalizable Conditions Pretest Selection Specificity of Variables Multiple Treatments Treatment Diffusion Experimenter Effects Reactive Arrangements/Participants Effects • History • Maturation • Testing – Pretest Sensitization • Instrumentation • Mortality • Statistical Regression • Differential Selection of Subjects • Selection-Maturation Interaction

  19. References Banks, J. A. (1993). Multicultural Education: Historical Development, Dimensions, and Practice.Review of Research in Education, 19, 3-49. doi: 10.3102/0091732X019001003Baker, C. (2006). Foundations of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism (4th ed.). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters Ltd. Baker, K. (1998). Structured English Immersion: Breakthrough in Teaching Limited- English- Proficient Students. Phi Delta Kappan, 80(3), 199-204. Retrieved from Eric database.  Bialystok, E., Luk, G., & Kwan, E. (2005). Bilingualism, Biliteracy, and Learning to Read: Interactions Among anguages and Writing Systems. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(1), 43-61. Retrieved from ERIC database. Coleman, R., & Goldenberg, C. (2009). What Does Research Say about Effective Practices for English Learners? Introduction and Part I: Oral Language Proficiency. Kappa Delta Pi Record, 46(1), 10-16. Retrieved from ERIC database.Duran, R. P. (2008). Assessing English-Language Learners’ Achievement. Review of Research in Education, 32, 92- 327. doi: 10.3102/0091732X07309372Garcia, O., & Baker, C. (Eds.). (2007). Bilingual Education An Introductory Reader. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters Ltd. Goldenberg, C. (2008). Teaching English Language Learners: What the Research Does—and Does not say. American Educator,32(2), 8-44. Retrieved from http://archive.aft.org/pubs-reports/american_educator/issues/summer08/goldenberg.pdf.Gort, M. (2006). Strategic codeswitching, interliteracy, and other phenomena of emergent bilingual writing: Lessons from first grade dual language classrooms. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 6, 323-354.doi: 10.1177/1468798406069796 Heredia, R. R., & Altarriba, J. (2001). Bilingual Language Mixing: Why Do Bilinguals Code-Switch? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10(5), 164-168. Retrieved from JSTOR database.Honigsfeld, A. (2009). ELL Programs: Not "One Size Fits All." Kappa Delta Pi Record, 45(4), 166-171. Retrieved from ERIC database.

  20. Hughes, C. E., Shaunessy, E. S., Brice, A. R., Ratliff, M. A., & McHatton, P. A. (2006). Code Switching among Bilingual and Limited English Proficient Students: Possible Indicators of Giftedness. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 30(1), 7-28. Retrieved from ERIC database.Leafstedt, J. M., & Gerber, M. M. (2005). Crossover of Phonological Processing Skills: A Study of Spanish-Speaking Students in Two Instructional Settings. Remedial and Special Education, 26, 226-235. doi: 10.1177/07419325050260040501Lee, J. W., & LemonnierSchallert, D. (1997). The Relative Contribution of L2 Language Proficiency and L1 Reading Ability to L2 Reading Performance: A Test of the Threshold Hypothesis in an EFL Context. TESOL Quarterly, 31(4), 713-739. Retrieved from JSTOR database. Lenters, K. (2004). No Half Measures: Reading Instruction for Young Second Language Learners. Reading Teacher, 58(4), 328-336. Retrieved from ERIC database. Macswan, J. (2000). The Threshold Hypothesis, Semilingualism, and Other Contributions to a Deficit View of Linguistic Minorities. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 22, 3-45. doi:10.1177/0739986300221001Martinez-Roldan, C. M., & Sayer, P. (2006). Reading through linguistic borderlands: Latino students’ transactions with narrative texts. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy,6, 293-322. doi: 10.1177/1468798406069799Miller, P., & Endo, H. (2004). Understanding and Meeting the Needs of ESL Students. Phi Delta Kappan, 85(10), 786-791. Retrieved from ERIC database.Mora, J. K. (2000). Staying the Course in Times of Change: Preparing Teachers for Language Minority Education. Journal of Teacher Education, 51, 345-357.doi:10.1177/0022487100051005003New York State Department of Education. (2008-09). The New York State School Report Card: District Summary Reports. Retrieved from New York City Department of Education Web site: https:www.nystart.gov Ochoa, S., & Rhodes, R. (2005). Assisting Parents of Bilingual Students to Achieve Equity in Public Schools. Journal of Educational and Psychological Consultation, 16 (1-2), 75-94. Retrieved from ERIC database.O’Connor-Petruso, S. A. (2010). Descriptive statistics and threats to validity [PowerPoint slides]. Retrieved from Brooklyn College Blackboard Web site: https://bbhosted.cuny.edu/Reese, L., Goldenberg, C., & Saunders, W. (2006). Variations in Reading Achievement among Spanish-Speaking Children in Different Language Programs: Explanations and Confounds. Elementary School Journal, 106(4), 363-385. Retrieved from ERIC database. Rolstad, K., Mahoney, K., & Glass, G. V. (2005). The Big Picture: A Meta-Analysis of Program Effectiveness Research on English Language Learners. Educational Policy, 19, 572-594.doi: 10.1177/0895904805278067

  21. Rossell, C. H., & Baker, K. (1996). The Educational Effectiveness of Bilingual Education.Research in the Teaching of English, 30(1), 7-74. Retrieved from Eric database. Sparks, R., Patton, J., Ganschow, L., & Humbach, N. (2009). Long-Term Crosslinguistic Transfer of Skills from L1 to L2. Language Learning, 59(1), 203-243. Retrieved from ERIC database. Tong, F., Lara-Alecio, R., Irby, B., Mathes, P., & Kwok, O. (2008). Accelerating Early Academic Oral English evelopment in Transitional Bilingual and Structured English Immersion Programs. American Educational Research, 45, 1011-1044. doi: 10.3102/0002831208320790Verdugo, R., & Flores, B. (2007). English-Language Learners: Key Issues.Education and Urban Society, 39, 167- 193. doi: 10.1177/0013124506294852Wiley, T G., & Wright, W. E. (2004). Against the Undertow: Language-Minority Education Policy and Politics in the "Age of Accountability." Educational Policy, 18, 142-168. doi: 10.1177/0895904803260030

More Related