1 / 27

Social and cognitive development

Social and cognitive development. Language evolution. special adaptations unique to human language? (Pinker & Bloom 1990) more general social and cognitive abilities allowing cultural learning? (Tomasello 2008, Csibra & Gergely 2009). Evidence for cultural learning.

Download Presentation

Social and cognitive development

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Social and cognitive development

  2. Language evolution • special adaptations unique to human language? (Pinker & Bloom 1990) • more general social and cognitive abilities allowing cultural learning? (Tomasello 2008, Csibra & Gergely 2009)

  3. Evidence for cultural learning • fixation on faces (Haith et al 1977, Farroni et al 2005) • increases over the first few weeks of life • triggered by child-directed speech • eye contact in response to caregiver’s vocalisations (Crown et al 2002) • neonates discriminate faces with eyes oriented towards them vs. with eyes looking away from them • the still-face effect: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=apzXGEbZht0 (Tronick et al, Nagy 2008)

  4. imitating facial expressions (Meltzoff & Moore 1977) • neonates can imitate facial expressions • and sounds accompanying facial expressions (/m/ versus /a/) (Chen et al 2004) • turn-taking behaviour before speech • vocalisations in response to non-human puppet vocalisations, taking turns

  5. Moving on • from dyadic interaction to triadic interaction • dyadic: child + caregiver • triadic: child + caregiver + the environment • referential communication: communications with others about the world

  6. Prerequisites to referential communication • gaze following: direction of caregivers gaze as a cue • 10 month-olds are more likely to follow head movements if the eyes are open (Brooks & Meltzoff 2005) • they pay more attention to an object • if another person is looking at them (Striano & Rochat 2000) (noticing communicative intentions) • if another person is also looking at the object (Striano et al 2006) (joint attention)

  7. Joint attention • infants’ ability to establish joint attention (by following adult gaze) predicts vocabulary size at 14-18 months (Carpenter et al 1998, Brooks & Meltzoff 2005) • for children with developmental disorders (Down Syndrome, ASD), joint engagement is a strong predictor of language development (Adamson et al 2009)

  8. interpreting pointing • understanding intentions • using shared knowledge • by 14 months infants can interpret pointing (Liebal et al 2009) (but this is later than the first words!!!)

  9. using pointing to reference objects • at 8-11 months (Carpendale & Carpendale 2010) • they point to draw attention to something, to seek help, to give directions, to request objects (Tomasello et al 2007)

  10. Advanced imitation • 12 month-olds copy even nonsense actions if that’s the intention of the adult (Nielsen 2006) • imitation of a nonsense action is more likely if the outcome is known in advance (Southgate et al 2009) • social learning, following convention language is also arbitrary, a matter of convention

  11. CONCEPTS, REPRESENTATION

  12. A representation of the world in memory • Deferred imitation task • adult performs an action, child is allowed to imitate the action after a period of delay • if they can do so, they must some kind of representation of the action • 9 month-olds can do the task (with 24 hour delay) (Meltzoff 1988) • Generalised imitation • adult performs an action using an object (e.g. gives a drink to an animal) • infant is asked to imitate the action using another object (another animal vs. a toy car) • at the 14 months infants appropriately generalise (Mandler & McDonough 1998)

  13. Playing with toys of different categories (animals versus vehicles) • 7 month-olds distinguish the categories (look at a toy from a new category for longer) (Mandler 2004) • Symbolic play • at 18 months they can pretend that one object is another

  14. The origin of conceptual categories • statistical learning from perceptual information? (Mareschal & Quinn 2001) • re-description of perceptual information? (Karmiloff-Smith 1992) • the role of language?

  15. WORD LEARNING

  16. Fenson et al 1994

  17. Fenson et al 1994

  18. 6-8 months: learn pairings of novel words with novel objects if the object is moved around as it is being named (Gogate et al 2010) • at 14 months they can learn labels without special attention seeking movement (Werker et al 1998) • toddlers associate novel words with novel objects or actions (Akhtar & Tomasello 1996, Tomasello et al 1996, Tomasello 2003)

  19. Errors • under-extension • word used for a specific objects rather than for the category (Bloom 1973) • over-extension • word used for broader category (Clark 1982)

  20. Vocabulary spurt • about one new word a day between 18 and 24 months • about 4 new words a day between 2 and 6 years • how do they do it?

  21. Mutual Exclusivity and the Principle of Contrast (Markman 1989, Clark 1993) • Children assume that a new word is the name of a new object or action • built-in constraint (Markman) • or general-purpose learning strategy (Regier 2003) • dogs appear to use this strategy (Kaminski et al 2004) • but children can learn multiple labels!!! • Ramscar (2011): distributed learning (ever finer grade differentiation)

  22. Whole object bias (Markman 1989, Hollich et al 2007) • we see the world in terms of whole objects (Spelke 194) • a connected and bounded region of solid matter • infants expect novel words to refer to a whole object category (Waxman & Markow 1995) • it’s not unique to humans (Winters et al 2008)

  23. The shape bias: dax (Landau et al 1988)

  24. The noun bias • noun meanings are easier to extend than verb meanings (Imai et al 2005) • language (and culture?) dependent • English speaking children tend to pick objects rather than actions as referents of novel words • Mandarin Chinese learners do not show this biassilent videos of Chinese and American mothers playing with their toddlers (Snedeker et al 2003) • Chinese and American adult viewers had to guess what the mothers were saying • viewers correctly identified more nouns than verbs in the American videos, but equal numbers of nouns and verbs in the Chinese videos

  25. Bayesian model of word learning (Xu & Tenenbaum 2007, Perfors et al 2011) • task: learn 24 object names (from 1 vs. 3 examples) • no word learning biases were built in • 45 objects were organised into a hierarchy of clusters based on their similarity ratings (based on anything: shape, size, colour, animacy, etc) • names were taught (egdalmatian for OBJECT8 and OBJECT13) • results: the model generalised the names the way 3-4 year-olds did.

  26. From syntax to word meaning • children use syntax to distinguish count nouns (a dax) from mass nouns (some dax) (Landau et al 1992) • argument structure can help identify verb meaning (The duck is gorping the bunny. vs. The duck and the bunny are gorping.) (Naigles 1990)

More Related