1 / 1

Students worked through two to four representations of each quiz.

End Notes. The effect of instructional environment on student representational competence. Patrick Kohl and Noah Finkelstein University of Colorado at Boulder per.colorado.edu. Current study and materials. Introduction. Previous work.

Download Presentation

Students worked through two to four representations of each quiz.

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. End Notes The effect of instructional environment on student representational competence Patrick Kohl and Noah Finkelstein University of Colorado at Boulder per.colorado.edu Current study and materials Introduction Previous work In a previous work,3 we began to investigate these questions. Students in a traditional large-lecture first-year physics course (Physics 202) were given homeworks with problem in four different representations (verbal, mathematical, graphical, pictorial). Students also received recitation quizzes that came in one of the four representational formats. Students in some recitation sections (choice group) were allowed to choose their format; others (control group) received one at random. We observed strong and statistically significant differences in performance between the choice and control groups on quizzes. Further, the direction of this effect (whether the choice or control group performed better) varied with representation and topic. • Student competence with different problem representations has been a subject of recent interest. Studies have included those of student competence with different representations1 and student meta-representational competence2 (what students know about representations). • Research Questions: • How does student performance vary with problem representation? Is facility with a representation constant across topics? • Are students aware of which representations they handle well (a meta-representational question)? What impact, if any, does this have on their performance? • How do different instructional methods affect students’ representational and meta-representational skills? • Repeat of previous study in Physics 201 and 202 taught by a reform-style professor. • These three courses taken together allow for comparison across course topic and instructional environment. • Example 201 quizzes are shown. Quiz questions and distractors mapped from one format to the next. In-depth interviews Course analysis Student performance data: Quizzes • We interviewed eight students from each of the Reform 201 and 202 classes. • Students solved a number of the study quizzes, and answered questions regarding the different representations and their formats. • Identical quizzes were given to each of the Traditional and Reform courses. Quizzes were written to match the specific material covered by the Traditional course; thus comparisons of absolute performance are likely invalid. • We show data from the two 202 courses to allow comparisons of the choice/control splits and performance variations across representation. The 201 data is not shown but is similar to the 202 in terms of relative performances, with higher absolute scores. • We analyzed traditional 202 and reform 201/202 courses in terms of the representational content of their lectures, exams, homeworks, and recitation/labs • Homeworks and labs were similar in representational content (though not necessarily in the use of those representations) • Lectures model representation use for students, exams hold students accountable for representational use: complementary course aspects Student solution strategies • Students worked through two to four representations of each quiz. • In many cases, student solution strategies varied strongly from representation to representation. In other cases, they were more consistent. We present examples of each. • Student TR solved mathematical, graphical, and pictorial versions of the pendulum quiz displayed above. Strategy summaries (in order of completion): • Mathematical: The pendulum is 4x as long but is pulled back 4x as far, and so will reach the same final position (incorrect). • Pictorial: The pendulum is pulled back to the same angle and thus won’t travel as far (student recalled a lecture demo where longer pendulums travel slower). TR selected the correct answer. • Graphical: The pendulum is 4x as long and so won’t travel as far; TR selected the correct answer. • Example: Student JS solved verbal, mathematical, and graphical versions of the pendulum quiz: • Verbal: Used to arrive at correct answer. • Mathematical: Used combination of above and v(t), x(t) equations to calculate and check correct answer. • Graphical: Used to arrive at correct answer. • Example problem from reform 201 course with mathematical and verbal components • Exams and lecture portions using more than one representation had those portions counted towards each relevant category; totals above 1.0 are possible Quiz performance data Results: Fraction of lectures using a representation Student assessments of their skills Results: Fraction of exams using a representation • The in-recitation study quizzes asked students which representational format they preferred to work in. We asked students this same question in the interviews. One out of 16 students contradicted their recitation answer in the interview; the rest did not. • We examine whether students perform better on the formats they prefer by examining all of the study problems they completed, broken into two groups: problems in representations that they described favorably, and problems in representations described neutrally or unfavorably. • Six of 15 students perform better on their preferred representations than other; 9 of 15 perform worse. This difference is not significant using a Wilcoxon signed-rank test. Statistical significant of choice/control splits (p-values) Conclusion Conclusion Conclusion • Choice/control splits appear to be influenced by the instructional environment; such splits are essentially absent from the reformed courses and are common in the traditional courses (see highlighted data, for example). • Hypothesis: Reform-style courses were richer in their use of representations, leading to development of broader student skills. In that case, receiving an assigned rather than a preferred representation would have less impact • Reform 201/202 courses used more representations and used multiple representations more often. • This could result in broader student representational skills, which could explain the observed student performance data. • Students’ quiz strategies often varied strongly with quiz representation, though some students are very consistent across representation. • Students generally appear to have robust opinions regarding the representations with which they are most competent; these opinions correlate poorly with their actual performances. Conclusions References • Student representational skills are influenced by: • Micro-level features (particular features of the problem or representation)3 • Macro-level features (the cumulative effect of instructional environment) • Students have fairly robust opinions of their own representational competence • These opinions are constant across contexts, while their skills are not necessarily • Pervasive use of different/multiple representations in instruction can have a noticeable positive effect on student skills Acknowledgements D. E. Meltzer. “Relation between students’.problem-solving performance and representational mode.” Am. J. Phys., 73:463, 2005. A. A. diSessa and B. L. Sherin. Meta-representation: an introduction. J. of Mathematical Behavior, 19:385, 2000 Kohl, P. B. and Finkelstein, N. D. “Representational Format, Student Choice, and Problem Solving in Physics.” Proceedings of the 2004 PERC (in press) • This work was supported in part by an NSF Graduate Fellowship and by Colorado PhysTEC. Special thanks to the rest of the Physics Education Research group at the University of Colorado at Boulder. Thanks also to Drs. Beale, Munsat, and Peterson and Noah Podolefsky for their cooperation and aid.

More Related