1 / 39

Connectionist Approaches to Language Acquisition

Connectionist Approaches to Language Acquisition. Kim Plunkett avec Julien Mayor, Jon-Fan Hu and Les Cohen Oxford BabyLab and UT, Austin. How to figure out the meaning of words…?. Huge number of possible meanings Hierarchical level? Relation between objects? Learning constraints?

neka
Download Presentation

Connectionist Approaches to Language Acquisition

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Connectionist Approaches to Language Acquisition Kim PlunkettavecJulien Mayor, Jon-Fan Hu and Les Cohen Oxford BabyLab and UT, Austin

  2. How to figure out the meaning of words…? • Huge number of possible meanings • Hierarchical level? • Relation between objects? • Learning constraints? e.g. whole object, taxonomic, …

  3. The weak ‘taxonomic constraint’ Given a choice between a thematically and a taxonomic related object, language users will favour the taxonomic choice over the thematic choice (Markman & Hutchinson 1984)

  4. The strong ‘taxonomic constraint’ « When infants embark upon the process of lexical acquisition, they are initially biased to interpret a word applied to an object as referring to that object and to other members of its kind » (Waxman & Markow 1995)

  5. Important implication of the TC From a single labelling event, infer that every object that belong to the same category is called with the same name • Powerful communication tool • Refer to objects one has never seen

  6. Controversy • Specifically linguistic? • How about the shape bias? • Innate? Learnt? Experimental status is unclear, e.g. Markman & Hutchison ’84 do not demonstrate the strong taxonomic constraint

  7. Objectives • We investigate how lexical organisation can use pre-lexical categorisation… • We want to understand the conditions necessary for having good generalisation of labels to object of like kinds (taxonomic constraint)… …within a modelling framework

  8. The model Baby • Early infancy; Baby gets experience with visual and acoustic environment • Later infancy, joint attentional activities with care-giver become important: => she gets simultaneous presentations of objects and labels

  9. The model of the model Baby! • Early infancy: unimodal maps (SOMs) receive input from visual and acoustic world (objects and labels) • Later infancy: object and their labels are presented at the same time. Synapses linking active neurones on both maps are reinforced

  10. Speech perception development • Initial sensitivity to speech… • prenatal exposure experiments (Mehler, Fifer & Moon 2003), fetal voice recognition (Kisilevsky 2003) • …to learning of language-specific sound patterns… • forward vs backward speech (Dehaene-Lambertz et al. 2002) • … and word segmentation • at 7-8m (Jusczyk & Aslin ‘95)

  11. Visual stream development • Basic structure of cortical maps innate but experience essential (Crair & al. 1998) • Contribution of sensory experience to development of orientation selectivity: • Stripped environment, alteration of dist. orientation preference (Blakemore & Cooper 2001) • Environment deprivation (White & al. 2001 )

  12. Modelling the unimodal perceptual development We use Self-Organising Maps (SOMs, Kohonen 1984) • they achieve dimensionality reduction • they self-organise around topological maps • they work in an unsupervised way (ie environment structures the maps) Similar objects are mapped to neighbouring neurons

  13. Visual input Prototype Blurred version (2/3 of max) Results reported on 20x20-sized images, 6 categories (cat, dog, cow, pig, sheep, bunny), 18 blurred images/prototype

  14. Early stage of infant life; passive presentation of images & words ‘Dog’ … ‘Sheep’ … Maps are structured, now we can build cross-modal associations

  15. After maps are structured; joint attentional activities Hebbian learning is ‘switched on’ (with n BMUs) ‘Dog’

  16. Testing procedure Label ? Does map structure support good generalisation? whole image dataset

  17. Random assignment Dog 1 Dog 2 Dog 3 Cat 1 Cat 2 Cat 3

  18. Memorisation (one-to-one mapping) Dog 1 Dog 2 Dog 3 Cat 1 Cat 2 Cat 3

  19. Successful generalisation Dog 1 Dog 2 Dog 3 Cat 1 Cat 2 Cat 3

  20. Generalisation after a single labelling event Classification success (%) Random One-to-one Simulation

  21. Role of # of pairings

  22. Developmental aspect of generalisation

  23. Role of map structure --- increasing map quality  Corr. of perceptual abilities with language (Tsao’04, Kuhl’05, …)

  24. Summary • In a first phase, we feed SOMs with realistic input, visual and acoustic • After maps are structured, we present a single word-object pair from a given class (e.g. dogs) • Generalisation of the label to other images in the same class (other dogs) is successful • We propose the taxonomic ‘constraint’ to be an emergent property of the network

  25. Role of # of Hebbian links

  26. Take-home messages • Map structure is critical for generalisation (similarity measure) • Good generalisation if many units are allowed to fire & wire together, even from single word-object presentation • If we reduce the number of units that fire & wire together, we restrict generalisation; role for proper nouns?

  27. Our neuro-computational account • Taxonomic responding is an emergent property of the network, inevitable outcome if exposed to the right environment, non-language specific • Architectural constraint: cross-modal association of two well-formed maps (early exposure to images and sounds) • Algorithmical constraint: activity-dependent learning (Hebbian type) • Mechanism for generalising associations between any formed categories

  28. Two Conditions Labelling Condition No Word Condition Labels facilitate CategorisationLabels are invitations to form categoriesWaxman and Colleagues Novelty Preference in Labelling Condition but not in No Word Condition

  29. Some Difficulties • Familiarisation stimuli probably from categories familiar to the infant – not necessarily category formation but category activation • Only one category is presented during familiarisation – category is not independently motivated by label • “No Word” condition is not a silent condition • Infants show “Out of Category” novelty preferences in the absence of labels • “No Word” condition is anomalous – perhaps overshadowing is occuring: Sloutsky et al.

  30. How to show that labels impact categories formation? • Show that a new visual category has been formed • Show that the structure of labelling events influences the structure of visual categories • Two possibilities • use labels to motivate category formation in the absence of visual structure • use labels to override existing visual categories to form a new category • Requires two labels and/or two categories

  31. Learning Novel Categories at 10-monthsYounger 1985 Familiarisation Testing

  32. > Novelty PreferenceYounger 1985 • Evidence for One category in Broad Condition: 1111/5555 > 3333 • Evidence for Two categories in Narrow Condition: 3333 > 1111/5555 >

  33. Impact of Labels on Categorisation in 10 month olds • Replicate Younger’s Original Experiments (No labels or carrier phrases) • Experiments 1 and 2 • Familiarise with Narrow Condition in 3 different labelling conditions: • Experiments 3 – 5 • Two labels correlated with category membership • Two labels uncorrelated with category membership • One label for all stimuli • Test for Novelty Preference • 24 Infants in each condition • Replication: 10 second familiarisation per trial – 4 test trials • Labelling: 6 second familiarisation per trial – 4 test trials • Testing conditions identical across all 5 experiments, i.e., in silence

  34. Familiarisation Findings • Hearing labels highlights attention to objects • Infants track perceptual similarity of objects

  35. Evidence for One category Evidence for Two categories Replication of Younger’s original findings(no label conditions)

  36. Evidence for Two categories No evidence for categories Evidence for One category Impact of labels on visual category structure – two categories

  37. Infants compute the cross-modal statistical correlations between labels and visual objects during the categorisation process Labels can override dissimilarities between objects so that they are treated as being perceptually more similar. No evidence for label facilitation No evidence for auditory dominance Conclusions

  38. Ongoing and Future Work • Can labels divide as well as unite? • Do words have a privileged status? • The impact of perceptual similarity • Integration with word learning

More Related