1 / 26

Latvian children’s realisations of word-initial consonant clusters

Latvian children’s realisations of word-initial consonant clusters. Sonority Sequencing, representational limits, and constraint reranking Martin Krämer & Olga Urek. Lava Lunch, 10 March 2017. Introduction.

juliand
Download Presentation

Latvian children’s realisations of word-initial consonant clusters

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Latvian children’s realisations of word-initial consonant clusters Sonority Sequencing, representational limits, and constraint reranking Martin Krämer & Olga Urek Lava Lunch, 10 March 2017

  2. Introduction • Which errors do children make in the L1 acquisition of complex word-initial onsets in Latvian? • Which role does sonority play in the acquisition of complex onsets? • Do established constraints and principles on complex onsets account for the data? • Does constraint reranking suffice to account for the attested variation? • Does the factorial typology of OT constraints provide a 100% match of attested patterns? • If it doesn’t, how do we fix this? • Which other factors play a role? Perceptual biases, articulatory challenges? • Data from the LAMBA phoneme testPicture naming task with children

  3. Participants • 20 monolingual Latvian-speaking children (mean age = 47 months, SD = 9.3); • Recruited in kindergartens in Riga, with Latvian as the only language of instruction; • Part of a larger study: • norming study for Latvian Phoneme Test (LAMBA); • 500 monolingual Latvian-speaking children total (36-72 months); • 20 children obtaining the lowest scores were selected;

  4. Procedure • Latvian Phoneme Test: • 91 pictures depicting objects and actions; • items chosen to contain all consonants and selected clusters in initial, medial and final position • Children tested individually in a quiet room by two evaluators; • Picture-naming procedure: • Ev. 1 showed the pictures to the child; • Ev. 2 marked the responses on the scoring sheet; • Each session was audio-recorded and later transcribed; • Spontaneous production was elicited where possible; • If the child could not name the picture spontaneously, delayed imitation was used;

  5. Data set: scope • All attempts at word-initial two-member clusters were extracted; • Six cluster types were considered: • stop + lateral (e.g. [bl-]) – 3 items • stop + rhotic (e.g. [tr-]) – 6 items • stop + nasal (e.g. [kn-]) – 1 item • s + lateral (e.g. [sl-]) – 2 items • s + nasal (e.g. [sn-]) – 2 items • s + stop (e.g. [st-]) – 5 items • NB: s + rhotic clusters are illicit in Latvian • Note: number of items per cluster type varies due to the structure of the Phoneme Test.

  6. Data set: coding • Child responses were coded to reflect the production of the attempted cluster: • fully correct: /sluota/  [sluota] ‘broom’ • fully omitted: /gredzens/  [edents] ‘ring’ • retained with segmental changes: /kru:ze/  [klu:ze] ‘cup’ • reduced to the less sonorous element: /blu:ze/[bu:de] ‘blouse’ • reduced to the more sonorous element: /snieks/  [nieks] ‘snow’ • A profile was generated for each child showing their production patterns for each cluster type:

  7. Data set: dealing with intra-speaker variation • Young children often demonstrate variable productions: i.e. not all inputs of the same type are consistently treated in the same way (e.g. Smith 1973, 2010) • Such variability is to be expected, since acquisition occurs gradually (Becker & Tessier 2010). In addition, both ‘precocious’ and ‘fossilized’ forms – i.e. individual ‘memorized’ words that do not reflect the current state of a child´s phonology – can contribute (Tessier 2009)

  8. Data set: dealing with intra-speaker variation • OT literature: intra-speaker variation – in children and adults – is the result of partial constraint ranking (Becker & Tessier 2010, Anttila 1997), i.e. the situation where dominance relation between two or more constraints is not fixed. • In such cases, unranked constraints can assume any mutual ranking each time a speaker attempts some production, which leads to variable results: Attempt A: Cluster reduction Attempt B: No reduction

  9. Data set: dealing with intra-speaker variation • In children, partial ranking is eventually resolved based on the positive evidence from the ambient language. • Crucially, the process is gradual: the proportion of non-target-like productions decreases slowly over time, and variability ‘dies out’ (see Becker & Tessier 2010 for technical implementation in OT). THEREFORE: • For children showing variation in their treatment of a given input: • if one of the patterns is preferred, we assumed it best reflects the state of child´s grammar, and disregarded the other one; • if no pattern is preferred, we assumed the existence of two grammar-states, and both were included into the analysis. • children who varied on more than one cluster type (N=2) were excluded.

  10. Data set • Thus, 18 children gave us the total of 27 observations or 12 patterns. Pattern APattern B • Of these, 9 followed Pattern A, 6 followed Pattern B, and the remaining 12 were unique. • Bad news: acquisition paths vary a lot more than theoretical literature would have you believe. • Good news: more than half of the data is accounted for by just 2 patterns. Definitely not random!

  11. The full typology

  12. Principles of syllable phonotactics • The Sonority Hierarchy (Sievers 1881, Jespersen 1904, Kiparsky 1979, Selkirk 1984, Clements 1990...) High V – G – L – N – F – T Low(V = vowels; G = glides; L = liquids; N = nasals; F = fricatives; T = stops) • Sonority Sequencing (Selkirk 1984, Clements 1990): ‘Within a syllable, sonority rises towards the peak and then falls.’ • Sonority Distance in onsets (Steriade 1982, Selkirk 1984, Levin 1985, Zec 2007) :‘Within an onset, a bigger rise in sonority is better.’TG > TL > TN > TT or {*TT, *TT/TN, *TT/TN/TL, *ComplexOnset} • Nucleus Hierarchy: ‘Higher sonority is better in the nucleus.’V > L > N > F > T or {*T/nuc, *T,F/nuc, *T,F,N/nuc, *C/nuc} • Margin hierarchy: ‘Low sonority is better in the onset.’T > F > N > L or {Onset, *G/ons, *G,L/ons, *G,L,N/ons, *G,L,N,F/ons}

  13. The appendix • Appendix is usually filled by only one segment, /s/ or /ʃ/ • Assumed to account for the sonority sequencing defying behaviour of sC onsets in many languages. • Some languages don’t allow “appendicitis”, e.g., Spanish (español). • Other languages allow appendix but no complex onsets • Spanish: *Appendix, Max >> Dep >> *ComplexOnsetOther languages:*ComplexOnset >> Max, Dep >> *Appendix

  14. Constraints • Defining the constraints that shape our typology according to perceived wisdom: • *Appendix: Assign a * for every onset with an appendix. • *ComplexOnset (*Cplx): Assign a * for every complex onset. • Sonority Distance (SD): Assign a * for every complex onset with a sonority distance ≤ 2 (*{ST,SN,TN} • OnsetHarmony (OH): *L, *L/N, *L/N/S • Max: Assign a * for every input segment that is not present in the output. • Max-Initial (Max-1): Assign a * for every input segment at the left edge that is not present in the output.

  15. Building the typology • To do a typology with OT Workplace, we abstract away from segmental changes.Sometimes L and R fluctuate.Sometimes only TL is present but not TR.Sometimes /pl/ is realized as [t], apparently coalescence rather than deletion. We count this nevertheless as reduction to least sonorant aka deletion of L. • We consider only deletion as an option. (No kid epenthesises vowels to break up consonant clusters anyway.) • Inputs: /SL, SN, ST, TL, TN/ • Input-output mappings/SL/ SL, S, L, S-L (hyphen = S in appendix)/SN/ SN, S, N, S-N/ST/ ST, S, T, S-T/TL/ TL, T, L/TN/ TN, T, N

  16. The typology

  17. The typology Phonetic target-like Target? GK GB GG Target? GF

  18. Problem zones in the attested typology

  19. Fixing problems – or what do the Latvian kids teach us about syllable phonotactics? • 8 Grammars undergenerated and 8 grammars overgenerated. • Overgeneration is not such a big deal: We had only 20 children. • Before we give up the Standard Theory: • What if syllabification is stratal? • Syllabification at Level I: Target-like • Add Faithfulness constraint on syllable structure to CON at level II.

  20. Stratal OT in a nutshell Lexicon /input/ Stem level Evaluation with Ranking 1 GEN provides candidates Winner of Level I = input to level II GEN provides candidates Word level Evaluation with Ranking 2 Winner of Level II = input to level III Phrasal level Evaluation with Ranking 3 GEN provides candidates [output]

  21. The other typology

  22. The other typology Colour legend: Green = targetlike Light green = attested lg. Yellow: Attested in our data That other colour: Phonetically the same as one of our patterns

  23. Fewer problem zones in the attested typology

  24. Fixing remaining problems – or what else do the Latvian kids teach us about syllable phonotactics? • 4 Grammars undergenerated and 17 grammars overgenerated. • Overgeneration is not such a big deal: We had only 20 children. • Resilience of N to deletion in TN /kn/ 1: Perception interferes with grammar.Nasality cues mask stop release and stop’s place cues, children don’t perceive k. • Resilience of N to deletion in TN /kn/ 2: Articulation interferes with grammar.Timing of stop gestures and velum lowering difficult to coordinate. • Resilience of N to deletion in TN /kn/ 3: Possibly coalescence, as in /pl/ -> [t],N is stop anyway. • Resilience of N to deletion in TN /kn/ 4: N is stop anyway. Misanalysed as “low sonority nasal” (Krämer & Zec 2016) • GI: (L, N, T, TL, N) = 2nd C wins. We could add Contiguity to Con.Overgenerates *(L, N, S, N, L) • GL: (T, SN, T, T, T) We still wonder what that child is doing here.

  25. Conclusions • The 20 weakest Latvian children produce patterns that • ...are not all attested in phonotactic typology • ...are not all generated by standard syllable theory. • Assuming stratal organisation of grammar and syllabification in the input to the last level matches results better, but overgenerates even more. • Though, we haven’t looked at complex onset tpyology in detail yet. • The remaining three child patterns can be explained by various grammatical and extragrammatical assumptions • Word of caution: We are looking at the worst out of 500. We’re potentially dealing with phonological pathologies, e.g., SLI.

  26. Thank you!

More Related