1 / 1

Michael Bersick (1), Doreen Nessler (1), Ray Johnson, Jr. (2), and David Friedman (1)

Fig. 6: After uninformative cues, the decrease in the amplitude of the target P3 was larger for unpredictable (b) than for predictable (a) pre-switch and switch trials. Fig. 7: (a) and (b): Target P3s to rare switch and pre-switch stimuli. Target onset. Baseline for cue- related ERPs. 300.

hedda-mccoy
Download Presentation

Michael Bersick (1), Doreen Nessler (1), Ray Johnson, Jr. (2), and David Friedman (1)

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Fig. 6: After uninformative cues, the decrease in the amplitude of the target P3 was larger for unpredictable (b) than for predictable (a) pre-switch and switch trials. Fig. 7: (a) and (b): Target P3s to rare switch and pre-switch stimuli. Target onset Baseline for cue- related ERPs 300 800 1300 0 500 1000 ms Cue onset Baseline for target-related ERPs Fig. 8: After uninformative cues, response conflict, as indicated by the amplitude of the MFN, was larger for unpredictable than predict-able, equiprobable switches. Single task Pre-switch Cz Switch 6a Predictable, informative cue Informative cue, unpredictable Cz Unpredictable, informative cue Informative cue, predictable Predictable, uninformative cue 6b Uninformative cue, unpredictable Unpredictable, uninformative cue 0 500 1000 ms 1a 1b Michael Bersick (1),Doreen Nessler (1), Ray Johnson, Jr. (2),and David Friedman (1) 1 Cognitive Electrophysiology Laboratory, NY State Psychiatric Institute, NY, NY;2 Department of Psychology , Queens College/CUNY, Flushing, NY Mixing Costs Switch Costs The cost of switching between two tasks is reduced by providing a cue warning of an impending switch. Cognitive control accounts of this effect claim that an informative cue allows for task-set reconfiguration prior to the actual switch [1]. Nevertheless, the fact that switch costs remain even after a long preparatory interval suggests that different task sets can interfere with each other [2]. However, such proactive interference might only be a problem when successive task sets must be reactivated. If switching is frequent, then keeping both task-sets active is a good strategy, assuming that little cost accrues from doing so. We examined how switch probability, switch predictability, and informative-cue availability interact to change the ease with which participants can maintain and access different task sets. Frequent change can be good for you: ERP evidence that task switch probability affects cognitive control Results Introduction Fig 2: Informative cues reduced mixing costs. Mixing costs decreased dramatically after uninformative cues in the equiprobable condition, but only when task order was predictable. Fig. 3: Informative cues reduced switch costs.In addition, when switches were equiprobable switch costs after uninformative cues were significantly reduced. Uninformative cue Informative cue Participants: Fifteen young adults (19 to 30 years; M = 24.6; SD = 3.8). Design: Digit (not 5) required response: more/less than 5? or odd/even? pure blocks: one task; mixed blocks: two tasks. Factors: (1) Ratio of switch (S) to stay or repeat (R) trials equiprobable; switch after 0, 1, or 2 trials: a-bb-aaa rare switch 1:3; switch after 2, 3, or 4 trials: aaa-bbbb-aaaaa (2) Cue-status: informative, uninformative (Fig. 1b); (3) Predictability Status: predictable, unpredictable Mixing costs = pre-switch RTs in mixed blocks - RTs in pure blocks Switch costs = switch RTs – pre-switch RTs mixed blocks EEG recording: 62 sintered Ag/AgCl electrodes; referred to averaged mastoids; continuous DC-100Hz; 500 Hz sampling rate; Fig. 1a: ERP epochs. Target P3 (a) Predictable MFN Methods Cz Fz Pz Pz 4a 7a 5a Equiprobable Rare switch Rare switch Cue-related P3 Equiprobable (b) Unpredictable 200 400 ms RT 8 Cz Pz Pz 4b Uninformative cue, predictable 5b 7b -500 0 500 1000 ms -1000 0 500 1000 ms -1000 -500 0 500 1000 ms cue onset target onset target onset Fig. 4:(b) Informative cues preceding equiprobable, unpredictable switches elicited a parietal, P3-like effect relative to pre-switch trials. (a) This effect was absent for informative cues on equiprobable, predictable switch trials. Fig. 5: (a) and (b) Informative cues preceding both predictable and unpredictable rare switches elicited much larger amplitude P3s than those present in the equiprobable condition (cf. Fig 4). Participants seem to utilize predictive information to actively maintain both task sets when doing so aids performance.Decreased mixing and switch costs for predictable, equiprobable stimuli, the decrease of cue-related P3 amplitude from rare to equiprobable switches, and the absence of any P3 to informative cues for equiprobable, predictable switches all suggest that cue information is increasingly irrelevant to performing the task as switches become more probable and predictable. Although response conflict was highest after uninformative cues, smaller target P3s for predictable vs. unpredictable equiprobable switches were observed, suggesting that predictability allowed processing resources to be conserved. Participants may also have created a more global behavioral set encompassing both tasks [3]. Switch costs in the equiprobable condition might be best described as relatively  “pure” costs of switching between active task sets rather than as proactive interference. Informative Uninformative 4 more/less than 5? Discussion ? 4 odd/even? References [1] Monsell & Mizon (2006). JEP: HPP(32), 493-516. [2] Wylie & Allport (2000). Psychological Research (63), 212-233. [3] Schneider & Logan (2006). JEP: General(135), 623-640.

More Related