1 / 18

Forward displacement, attention and the multiple-object tracking (MOT) task

Forward displacement, attention and the multiple-object tracking (MOT) task. Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge, School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen. Introduction 1. MOT task (Pylyshyn & Storm, 1988)

elwyn
Download Presentation

Forward displacement, attention and the multiple-object tracking (MOT) task

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Forward displacement, attention and the multiple-object tracking (MOT) task Roy Allen & Peter McGeorge, School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen

  2. Introduction 1 • MOT task (Pylyshyn & Storm, 1988) • Expertise (e.g., Allen et al., 2004; Barker et al., 2010; Green & Bavalier, 2006a, 2006b); • Developmental (e.g., Trick et al., 2005); • What of trajectory information? • Extrapolate target location (Keane & Pylyshyn, 2006): • All items disappeared for 150-900ms; • Performance best when items reappeared at point where they disappeared or back along trajectory • Poorest when items reappeared forward of vanishing point. • But Fencsik et al. (2007): • All items disappeared for 300ms; • As well as successfully tracking 4 items observers also held motion information for two items.

  3. Introduction 2 • Like Forward Displacement Bias debate (Finke & Freyd, 1985): • Single object with actual or implied motion, disappears; • Observers misjudge spatial location of disappearance; • Seen as index of ability to extrapolate location from trajectory information; • Subsequently seen as influenced by many factors (e.g., direction of travel, speed, nature of object etc. – see Hubbard, 2005). • Role of attention (Hayes & Freyd, 2002) (implied motion): • Dual task, manipulated probability that one or other of two objects would be probed; • Found when attention had to be divided over both objects (i.e., when probability was 20% or 35%) forward displacement greater compared with when attention more focused (i.e., when probability was 65% or 80%). • But Kerzel (2003) (implied motion); McGeorge et al. (2006): • Both suggested attention was necessary for a forward displacement bias to occur.

  4. Introduction 3 • Alvarez and Franconeri (2007) • Proposed a resource-limited model of MOT - as the number of items to be tracked increases, the attentional resources allocated to each item decreases: • This leads to a reduction in tracking accuracy (see also Barker et al., 2010); and, • if we accept the suggestion that attention is necessary to extrapolate target location from trajectory information, as target numbers increase forward displacement bias will decrease. • We investigated this latter prediction in our first experiment

  5. Method 1 • 34 participants (10 female) aged from 17 – 29 (M = 20.29, SD = 2.59) • 144 trials

  6. Method 2 Perceived target Position Final target Position fd Target trajectory at time of disappearance

  7. Results 1 • Discrimination Accuracy:A repeated-measures ANOVA, of the target data, was carried out with Delay (no delay, delay), and Number of targets to be tracked (2, 3, 4, 5) as the within-subjects factors; • There was a main effect of Delay (F(1, 33) = 28.99, p < .001, ηp2 = .47) (No Delay: M = 79.82, SD = 1.60; Delay: M = 70.35, SD = 2.22); • There was also a main effect of Number of targets to be tracked (F(3, 99) = 29.78, p < .001, ηp2 = .47). The interaction was not significant.

  8. Results 2 • Forward Displacement • A repeated-measures ANOVA, on the target data, was carried out with Delay (no delay, delay), and Number of targets to be tracked (2, 3, 4, 5) as the within-subjects factors; • There were significant main effects of Type and Number of targets to be tracked. These were moderated by a significant interaction of Type x Number of targets to be tracked (F(1.97, 64.95) = 5.52, p = .006, ηp2 = .14);

  9. Results 3 • Paired-samples t-tests (p = .013 for multiple comparisons) showed that performance in the No Delay and Delay conditions varied significantly for 2 and 3 targets (2 targets: t33 = 4.32, p < .001; 3 targets: t33 = 2.68, p = .011); • One-sample t-tests showed that for both 2 and 3 targets, in the No Delay condition, forward displacements were significantly greater than zero (2 targets: t33 = 2.09, p = .044; 3 targets: t33 = 2.33, p = .026).

  10. Discussion • Results show forward displacement for small numbers of targets in the No delay condition (supports Fencsik et al., 2007); • Supports idea that attention may be a necessary component of generating a forward displacement bias (Kerzel, 2003: McGeorge et al., 2006) (i.e., in terms of Alvarez and Franconeri (2007) idea of a resource-limited model of MOT, as attention divided amongst more targets so displacement bias disappears); •  Finally, trajectory knowledge appears relatively short-lived (i.e., no forward displacement bias following a1000ms delay); • However, is it just an object’s motion that influences FD, or can what we know about an object also be influential? We investigate this in experiment 2.

  11. Experiment 2 • Reed and Vinson (1996, exp 1) contrasted the FD bias shown when participants thought they were viewing either a single rocket or a steeple. • While these two stimuli had the same visual characteristics they have very different typical motions – rockets move, steeples rarely do. • Found that participants, told that the stimulus represented a steeple, showed significantly smaller FD biases than participants told that the same stimulus was a rocket, particularly for implied upward motion. • Greater FD bias for an object that moved, relative to one that did not, is consistent with the use of knowledge of the object’s typical motion to predict its position. • The fact that this bias was found to be greatest when looking at implied upward motion (the typical motion of a rocket), underlines this point. • Knowledge, it appears, influences how we perceive motion (real or implied), and, thus, how easy it is to predict where an object will move, making it easier to track. • Does this effect extend to MOT?

  12. Method/Results • 21 participants (11 female), ages ranged from 18 – 25 (M = 20.4, SD = 4.5). • Paradigm was same as experiment 1, but stimuli were either all butterflies or all flowers (pansies). The former are, typically, expected to move in the random way MOT items move. • Discrimination Accuracy:A repeated-measures ANOVA, of the target data, was carried out with Class (butterflies, pansies), and Number of targets to be tracked (2, 3, 4, 5) as the within-subjects factors; • There were main effects of Class and Number of targets to be tracked. These were moderated by a Class x Number of targets to be tracked interaction (F(3, 60) = 10.39, p < .001, ηp2 = .34).

  13. Results 2 • Paired-sample t-tests showed that discrimination accuracy for butterflies, on 5-target trials, was significantly greater than that for pansies (t20 = 4.93, p < .001). • Forward displacement: • A repeated-measures ANOVA was carried out, on target data, with Class (Butterfly, Pansy) and Number of targets to be tracked (2, 3, 4, 5) as the within-subjects factors. • Main effects of Class and Number of targets to be tracked were moderated by a significant Class x Number of targets to be tracked interaction (F(3, 60) = 10.22, p < .001, ηp2 = .34).

  14. Results 3 • Paired-sample t-tests (p set to .016 for multiple comparisons) showed that butterflies and pansies only differed significantly for 2-target and 4-target trials (2 targets: t20 = 3.53, p = .002; 3 targets: t20 = 0.16, p = .878; 4 targets: t20 = 4.67, p < .001). • For pansies, one-sample t-tests show forward displacement for two and three targets is significantly greater than zero (2 targets: t20 = 5.36, p < .001; 3 targets: t20 = 2.28, p = .033) • For butterflies, forward displacement for three and four targets is significantly greater than zero (2 targets: t20 = 0.22, p = .828; 3 targets: t20 = 6.81, p < .001; 4 targets: t20 = 4.80, p <.001).

  15. Conclusions • Experiment 1 confirmed Fenscik et al.’s (2007) claim that observers’ can retain trajectory information about circa 2-3 target objects; • Also showed that such trajectory information decays and is gone by circa 1000ms; • Supports idea that attention may be a necessary component of generating a forward displacement bias (Kerzel, 2003: McGeorge et al., 2006) (i.e., in terms of Alvarez and Franconeri (2007) idea of a resource-limited model of MOT, as attention divided amongst more targets so displacement bias disappears). • Experiment 2 showed differences in tracking (butterflies tracked better than pansies) and trajectory information for butterflies and pansies (2-3 for pansies; 3-4 for butterflies); but, • No trajectory information for 2-target butterfly trials? Suggests the expectation that a type of object moves directs increased attention to them. Suggests attention necessary to prevent fd when objects expected to move.

  16. Other analyses • Location Accuracy • Significant main effect of number and class x number interaction that approached significance (F(1.58, 31.71) = 3.46, p = .054, ηp2 = .15) • Paired-sample t-tests showed butterfly accuracy, on 2-target trials, was significantly greater than for pansies (2 targets: t20 = 3.51, p = .002)

  17. References 1 • Allen, R., McGeorge, P., Pearson, D. G., & Milne, A. B. (2004). Attention and expertise in multiple target tracking. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 337 - 347. • Alvarez, G. A. & Franconeri, S. L. (2007). How many objects can you track?: Evidence for a resource-limited attentive tracking mechanism. Journal of Vision 7(13):14, 1 – 10. • Barker, K., Allen, R., & McGeorge, P. (2010) Multiple-Object Tracking: Enhanced visuospatial representations as a result of experience. Experimental Psychology, 57, 208 - 214. • Fencsik, D. E., Klieger, S. B. & Horowitz, T. S. (2007). The role of location and motion information in the tracking and recovery of moving objects. Perception & Psychophysics, 69 (4), 567-577. • Finke, R. A. & Freyd, J. J. (1985). Transformations of visual memory induced by implied motions • of pattern elements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 11, 780 - 794. • Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2006a). Enumeration versus multiple object tracking: The case of action video game players. Cognition, 101, 217 - 245. • Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2006b). Effect of action video games on the spatial distribution of visuospatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32 (6), 1465 - 1478. • Hayes, A. E. & Freyd, J. J. (2002). Representational momentum when attention is divided. Visual Cognition, 9 (1/2), 8 - 27.

  18. References 2 • Hubbard, T. L. (2005). Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings. Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12 (5), 822 - 851. • Keane, B. P., & Pylyshyn, Z.W. (2006). Is motion extrapolation employed in multiple object tracking? Tracking as a low level no-predictive function. Cognitive Psychology, 52, 346 - 368. • Kerzel, D. (2003). Attention maintains mental extrapolation of target position: Irrelevant distractors eliminate forward displacement after implied motion. Cognition, 88, 109 - 131 • McGeorge, P., Beschin, N. & Della Sala, S. (2006). Representing target motion: The role of the right hemisphere in the forward displacement bias. Neuropsychology, 20(6), 708 - 715. • Pylyshyn, Z., & Storm, R. W. (1988). Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial Vision, 3 (3), 179 - 197. • Reed, C.L., & Vinson, N.G. (1996). Conceptual effects on representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(4), 839 - 850. • Trick, L. M., Jaspers-Fayer, F., & Sethi, N. (2005). Multiple-object tracking in children: The ‘‘catch the spies’’ task. Cognitive Development, 20, 373 - 387.

More Related