1 / 14

Refined Micro-analysis of Fluency Gains in a Reading Tutor that Listens

Refined Micro-analysis of Fluency Gains in a Reading Tutor that Listens. Jack Mostow* and Joseph Beck Project LISTEN ( www.cs.cmu.edu/~listen ) Carnegie Mellon University * Consultant and Scientific Advisory Board Chair, Soliloquy Learning Society for the Scientific Study of Reading

belles
Download Presentation

Refined Micro-analysis of Fluency Gains in a Reading Tutor that Listens

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. RefinedMicro-analysis of Fluency Gains in a Reading Tutor that Listens • Jack Mostow* and Joseph Beck • Project LISTEN (www.cs.cmu.edu/~listen) • Carnegie Mellon University * Consultant and Scientific Advisory Board Chair, Soliloquy Learning • Society for the Scientific Study of Reading • 13th Annual Meeting, July, 2006 • Funding: National Science Foundation, Heinz Endowments 1

  2. Research questions and approach • Guided oral reading builds fluency [NRP 00] • Typically repeated oral reading • … but how do its benefits vary? • How good is repeated vs. wide reading? • How good is massed vs. spaced practice? • How do the answers vary with student proficiency? • Approach: micro-analyze oral reading data • Massive: hundreds of children • Longitudinal: entire school year • Fine-grained: word by word 2

  3. Massive 650 students age 5-14 Mostly grades 1-4 Longitudinal 2003-2004 school year 55,000 sessions Fine-grained 6.9 million words “Heard” by recognizer Video at www.cs.cmu.edu/~listen Project LISTEN’s Reading Tutor: Rich source of guided oral reading data 3

  4. Reading speeds up with practice: example • Initial encounter of muttered: I’ll have to mop up all this (5630 ms) muttered Dennis to himself but how • 5 weeks later (different word pair in different sentence): Dennis (110 ms) muttered oh I forgot to ask him for the money • Word reading time = latency + production time  1/fluency • How does word reading time change in general? 4

  5. Do some types of encounters help more than others? Learning curve for mean reading time of first 20 encounters, excluding top 50 words 5

  6. Four types of word encounters • Predict reading time for 770,858type 1 encounters • from prior encounters of all 4 types. 6

  7. Predictor variables • Number of word encounters so far of each type • Wide vs. reread • Spaced vs. massed • Word difficulty • # of letters • # of past help requests (controls for difficulty for that student) • Student proficiency • WRMT Word Identification grade-equivalent score, e.g. 2.3 • Interpolated for each encounter from pre- and post-test scores 7

  8. Exponential model of word reading time • = L * # letters + (P * proficiency + constant A) * e - learning rate B *Exposure • Define weights for each type of encounter • rfor rereading vs. 1 for wide reading • mfor massed vs. 1for spaced • h for help requests • Exposure = weighted sum of # of word encounters so far • 1 * # of wide, spaced encounters • + r * # of reread, spaced encounters • + m * # of wide, massed encounters • + r * m * # of reread, massed encounters • + h * # of help requests • [Beck, J. Using learning decomposition to analyze student fluency development. ITS2006 Educational Data Mining Workshop, Taiwan.] 8

  9. Analysis • Use SPSS non-linear regression to fit parameters • Caveat: 770,858 trials are not independent • So be conservative: • Split 650 students into 10 groups • Fit r, m, … for each group • From the 10 estimates of each parameter, compute: • Mean ± standard error • Differs significantly from 1? 9

  10. Overall results • Wide reading beats rereading • r = .68 ± .13 • r < 1 (p = .007) • 2 new stories ≈ 3 old stories • Spaced beats massed practice • m = .67 ± .13 • m < 1 (p = .007) • 2 spaced encounters ≈ 3 massed encounters • Do these results vary by proficiency? 10

  11. When does wide reading beat rereading? Maybe only for high readers? Seeing a word again the same day May help low readers more than waiting (p = .058) Helps higher readers less than seeing it later (p < .01) Effects of proficiency 11

  12. Conclusion: type of practice matters! • Wide reading beats rereading • At least for higher readers • Advantage of spaced practice varies with proficiency • Low readers: seeing a word again the same day may help more • Higher readers: better to wait • Fluency growth is slow (learning curve is gradual) • So differences in practice quality are hard to detect • But possible by micro-analysis of massive, longitudinal, fine-grained data • Future work • Clarify interaction with proficiency • Refine model of fluency practice • Test correlational results experimentally • Thank you! Questions? • See papers & videos at www.cs.cmu.edu/~listen 12

  13. Predictive models of word reading in text 13

  14. Outcome variable • Combine reading time, errors, help requests • Cap reading time at 3 seconds (0.1% of data) • Treat error as 3 seconds • Treat help request as 3 seconds 14

More Related