1 / 28

1954 Salk polio vaccine trials

1954 Salk polio vaccine trials. Biggest public health experiment ever Polio epidemics hit U.S. in 20 th century Struck hardest at children Responsible for 6% of deaths among 5- to 9- year-olds. Salk vaccine trial: Background. Polio is rare but the virus itself is common

misu
Download Presentation

1954 Salk polio vaccine trials

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. 1954 Salk polio vaccine trials Biggest public health experiment ever Polio epidemics hit U.S. in 20th century Struck hardest at children Responsible for 6% of deaths among 5- to 9- year-olds

  2. Salk vaccine trial: Background • Polio is rare but the virus itself is common • Most adults experienced polio infection without being aware of it. • Children from higher-income families were more vulnerable to polio! • Children in less hygienic surroundings contract mild polio early in childhood while still protected from their mother’s antibodies. They develop immunity early. • Children from more hygienic surroundings don’t develop such antibodies.

  3. Salk trial: The need for testing • By 1954, Salk’s research with a vaccine looked promising • Government agencies were ready to try the vaccine in the general population but some scientists feared the vaccine was unsafe or ineffective. • There was enormous fear and desperation throughout the country. • Why not just distribute the vaccine to some and see if it lowered the polio rate? • A yearly drop might mean the drug was effective, or that that year was not an epidemic year • Vaccine could not be distributed without testing

  4. Salk vaccine trial: The need for controls • An experiment requires controls. • To test if the vaccine was effective the only variable that should be considered is the vaccine itself • This means that some children would get the vaccine and some would not. • This raises enormous ethical questions: • Is it ethical to not give children the vaccine? • Imagine yourself as a parent in these desperate times. Would you participate in such an experiment. • Ultimately, does the benefit to society outweigh the risk to those children who would not get the vaccine?

  5. Salk vaccine:The need for massive trials • Polio rate of occurrence is about 50 per 100,000 • Suppose the vaccine was 50% effective and 10,000 subjects were recruited for each of the control and treatment groups • You would expect 5 polio cases in control group and 2-3 in treatment group • Such a difference could be attributed to random variation • Clinical trials were needed on a massive scale • The ultimate experiment involved over 1.6 million children, with over 600,000 children inoculated

  6. Controversy over the design of the experiment • In order to isolate the vaccine as the only variable to be considered, the treatment and control groups need to be as similar as possible • But how should subjects be recruited? • Fact: volunteers tend to be better educated and more well-to-do than those who don’t participate • In the context of the polio disease, relying on volunteers could potentially bias the results • Subjects would tend to have higher rates of polio • Subjects are not representative of the population • Results would be biased against the vaccine • After much debate, the trials proceeded with two different protocols.

  7. “Observed Control” approach • Administer the experiment to 1st, 2nd, and 3rd graders • Offer the vaccination to 2nd graders • This group would rely on volunteers (parental consent) • Use 1st and 3rd graders as control group • These children would be observed for incidences of polio • Supporters of this approach argued that there would not be much variability between grades so treatment and control groups would be similar • And the control group would be “observed controls” • But there were objections . . .

  8. NFIP Observed Control study Volunteers would result in more children from higher income families in treatment group Treatment group is thus more vulnerable to disease than control group Would expect more incidences of polio in the treatment group than in the control group Biases the experiment against the vaccine How would incidents of the disease be diagnosed? Many forms of polio are hard to diagnose In making the diagnosis physicians would naturally ask whether a child was vaccinated or not Diagnosis for borderline cases could be affected by knowledge of what grade the child was in and whether the child was vaccinated or not

  9. Randomized control approach • This experiment relied on volunteer subjects overall. • But subjects were randomly assigned to treatment and control groups • Control group was given a placebo • Placebo material was prepared to look exactly like the vaccine so subjects didn’t know what treatment they were getting • Placebo-control group guards against the “placebo effect” • Many objected to the design on ethical grounds. • Jonas Salk himself called it “A `beautiful’ experiment over which the epidemiologist could become quite ecstatic but which would make the humanitarian shudder.”

  10. Randomized control approach • Subjects were “blind”: they did not know to which group they were assigned • Also, those doing the evaluation didn’t know which treatment any subject received • Each vial was identified by a code number so no one involved in the vaccination or the diagnostic evaluation could know who got the vaccine. • Experiment was double-blind: neither subjects nor those doing the evaluation knew which treatment any subject received

  11. Results of vaccine trials The randomized, controlled experiment The Observed Control study Source: Thomas Francis, J r., “An evaluation of the 1954 Poliomyelitis vaccine trials---summary report,” American Journal of Public Health vol 45 (1955) pp. 1-63.

  12. Comparing the two studies • Results show that the observed control study was biased against vaccine • Treatment group got the vaccine but was more prone to higher polio rates • Control group didn’t get the vaccine but was more prone to lower polio rates • It’s impossible to determine what’s the effect of the vaccine and what’s the effect of socio-economic status • This is called confounding—the inability to distinguish the separate impacts of two or more variables on a single outcome. • In a randomized controlled experiment, by making the treatment and control groups as similar as possible (by randomization), we are able to isolate the variable of interest and eliminate confounding

  13. Comparing the two studies: are the results “significant”? • In the “observed control” approach, chance enters the study in an unplanned and haphazard way based on what families will volunteer • By contrast, for the randomized controlled experiment chance enters the study in a planned and simple way • Each child has 50-50 chance to be in the treatment or control group • This allows for the use of probability to analyze the results

  14. Are the results significant? • Two competing positions—which side would you be on? • Pro: “The vaccine is effective. There were less cases of polio in the treatment group than in the control group. We should undertake a massive vaccination program throughout the general population.” • Con: “We are not convinced. The two groups were randomly divided. There may have been fewer polio-prone people in the treatment group. It was all done by chance. We can’t be sure and we’re not willing to commit millions of dollars of taxpayer’s money on a vaccination program that might not be effective.”

  15. Are the results significant? • Assume the cons are right and that the vaccine is worthless. What are the chances of seeing such a large difference in the two groups? • Imagine a “polio” coin where the chance of heads is equal to the chance that a person gets polio. Flip the coin in Room A for 200,000 times. Then flip it in Room B for 200,000 times. What’s the chance that we would get such a large difference as 28 heads in A and 71 heads in B? • They are over a billion to one against! • In the face of such odds, we say that the outcome is statistically significant. The effect is so large that it would rarely occur by chance.

  16. Salk vaccine trials aftermath • The results, announced in 1955, showed good statistical evidence that Jonas Salk's vaccine was 80-90% effective in preventing paralytic poliomyelitis. • Postscript: Polio was virtually eliminated from the Americas in 1994, but still circulates in Asia and Africa, paralyzing the world’s most vulnerable children. • The Global Polio Eradication Initiative was begun in 1988. That year, an estimated 350,000 children were paralyzed with polio worldwide. • In 2004, polio cases had fallen to just over 1,200 cases globally.

  17. The language of experimental design • In an experiment, we have at least one explanatory variable, called a factor, to manipulate and at least one response variable to measure • The specific values that the experimenter chooses for a factor are called the levels of the factor. • A treatment is a combination of specific levels from all the factors that an experimental unit receives. • The ability to manipulate factors, apply treatments, and compare the responses is what differentiates an experiment from an observational study

  18. Observational studies • Nurses Health Study often in the news • Over 100,000 registered nurses aged 30 to 55 have been followed for more than 30 years • Detailed questionnaires sent out every two years on a wide variety of health and nutrition issues • 90% response rate • “One of the most significant studies ever conducted on the health of women.” -- Donna Shalala, Former Secretary of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services • This is a prospective study. Subjects were identified in advance and data collected as events unfolded. • Many observational studies are retrospective. Subjects are selected and their previous conditions or behaviors are determined.

  19. Confounding • Observational studies can suffer from confounding and lurking variables • You’ll read about this over the weekend in “Hormone Studies: What Went Wrong?” • The ability to control and manipulate variables and compare groups allows for eliminating confounding and the effect of lurking variables

  20. Double-blind, placebo-controlled randomized comparative experiment: The “gold standard” of statistics • Massive clinical trials industry • Complex ethical questions for experiments involving human subjects • Informed Consent, Institutional Review Board, Confidentiality • Placebo effect is a fascinating area of research • In conditions such as pain, the percent of patients responding to placebos has been shown to be 20% to 50%. • Reflects the amount that the body can be coaxed/empowered to heal itself, in the absence of other active agents. • Today, few clinical trials compare against placebo. Most new drugs are improvements over existing therapies. If an existing medicine exists it would be unethical to deny it to subjects

  21. Other experimental design issues: Blocking • When groups of experimental units are similar, it’s often a good idea to gather them together into blocks. • Blocking isolates the variability due to the differences between the blocks so that we can see the differences due to the treatments more clearly. • When randomization occurs only within the blocks, we call the design a randomized block design • By contrast, a completely randomized design, all subjects have an equal chance of receiving any treatment.

  22. Diagram of a blocked experiment

  23. Hypertension pharmacogenetics study • Hypertension is most prevalent risk factor for diseases of the heart, brain and kidneys, affecting 43 million in U.S. • Complex disease affected by physical, physiological and environmental factors • State-of-the-art for treatment is trial-and-error • Less than 40% of treated patients achieve blood pressure control (systolic blood pressure < 140) • Ultimate goal of this study is to identify unknown genes that influence drug response with the potential of tailoring antihypertensive therapy for individuals

  24. GERA Clinical Trial • Black and white patients react differently to blood pressure medicine • Blocked experimental design • Mayo Clinic – Rochester, MN • 300 white subjects with hypertension (150 women and 150 men, ages 30 to 60) • Emory University – Atlanta, GA • 300 Black subjects with hypertension (150 women and 150 men, ages 30 to 60) • Subjects had previous medications discontinued for 4 weeks; blood pressure rose and stabilized in hypertensive range • Hydrochlorothiazide administered for 4 weeks • Blood pressure measured at the beginning of therapy and after 4 weeks • In each group, identify 100 “best” responders and 100 “worst” responders by change in blood pressure

  25. BP decrease BP increase YrN Race Drug Race N 1 100 B Hydrochlorothiazide B 100 2 100 W W 100

  26. GERA clinical trial • DNA collected for each patient • Data consists of 100,000 genetic markers called Single-Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs) • Goal: to find an association between blood pressure response and genetic makeup • Ultimate goal: to find those genes that affect blood pressure response • What makes this complicated is that we have only 400 observations (the patients) and over 100,000 variables (the genetic markers) • Classically in statistics we had a “few” variables and “many” observations. As datasets become larger and more complex, this classic paradigm is shifting and the challenges are enormous!

More Related