1 / 16

Embedding Behavior Change in its Social & Physical Context

Embedding Behavior Change in its Social & Physical Context. Carol M. Werner University of Utah USA Paper presented at NAHMMA, 2005, Tacoma WA. Environmental Behavior Change. No Silver Bullet Holistic approach individual & supportive context social milieu (friends, society)

Download Presentation

Embedding Behavior Change in its Social & Physical Context

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Embedding Behavior Change in its Social & Physical Context Carol M. Werner University of Utah USA Paper presented at NAHMMA, 2005, Tacoma WA

  2. Environmental Behavior Change • No Silver Bullet • Holistic approach individual & supportive context social milieu (friends, society) political/economic system physical environment

  3. Social Milieu • Perceived opinions of: Immediate Friends/Family Larger Social milieu TV, radio, print: advertising, commentary • Social Pressures: “Pluralistic ignorance” (disagree, but fear rejection) Hearing others endorse new behavior gives the individual “permission” to change.

  4. Creating positive social milieu: A route to individual attitude change • Guided group discussions (Lewin) • Not a lecture: • Group members endorse new idea • Group members discuss problems and solutions • Leader guides discussion in support of nontoxics

  5. Results: Community Groups:Own Opinions • Table 1. Attitudes and Behaviors Since the Meeting • ALL ORGANIZERS MATCHED SAMPLE (n = 46) • PERSONAL ATTITUDES AND BEHAVIORSa Organizer Control n • Took things to HHW facility? 33% 35% 10%*20 • Shared leftovers? 36% 35% 12%*17 • Begin/continue sharing? 6.6 5.3 3.6*18 • Important to reduce use 9.7 9.7 9.0*23 • Plan to use nontoxicsb8.4 8.7 7.2*23 • 11-points scales, 6=middle point • Matched sample did not hear information. This difference shows people learned something at the meeting. • * matched groups differ at p < .05, 1-tailed dependent t-tests • Column one shows responses of all 46 organizers (for comparison to the reduced sample). Columns two and three show the subgroup of organizers with their matched controls; n’s for the subgroups are in parentheses; statistical tests compare the subgroup of 23 organizers with their matched controls. • a Percentages indicate percent of respondents saying “yes” to that item. Other items were rated on 1-11 scales, with ends labeled “Extremely Unlikely/Extremely Likely,” “Extremely Unimportant/Extremely Important,” or “Extremely Unsatisfied/Extremely Satisfied.” • b Mean of three items: 1) likely to use more nontoxics around the home; 2) likely to use nontoxics to care for landscaping; and 3) satisfaction with nontoxic alternatives.

  6. Results: Community Groups:Predicting Friends’ Opinions • Table 1. Attitudes and Behaviors Since the Meeting • ALL ORGANIZERS MATCHED SAMPLE (n = 46) • ESTIMATES OF GROUP’S • ATTITUDES AND BEHAVIOR Organizers Control n • Group valued meeting 9.4 9.2 Not asked 23 • Group shared leftovers 24% 27% “Don’t Know” 22 • Group begin/continue sharing? 5.7 6.0 4.2* 12 • IMPORTANCE OF “BEING AT MEETING” • Matched sample could not predict friends’ opinions • * matched groups differ at p < .05, 1-tailed dependent t-tests • Column one shows responses of all 46 organizers (for comparison to the reduced sample). Columns two and three show the subgroup of organizers with their matched controls; n’s for the subgroups are in parentheses; statistical tests compare the subgroup of 23 organizers with their matched controls. • a Percentages indicate percent of respondents saying “yes” to that item. Other items were rated on 1-11 scales, with ends labeled “Extremely Unlikely/Extremely Likely,” “Extremely Unimportant/Extremely Important,” or “Extremely Unsatisfied/Extremely Satisfied.” • b Mean of three items: 1) likely to use more nontoxics around the home; 2) likely to use nontoxics to care for landscaping; and 3) satisfaction with nontoxic alternatives.

  7. ? RelevantAttitude Discussionchange WHY? MORE PERSUASION? MORE LEARNING? ACTIVE LEARNING? PERCEIVED GROUP ENDORSEMENT?

  8. Replication: High School Classes • True experiment (“causality”): Lecture vs. Guided Discussion Random assignment to treatment • Is guided group discussion more effective than “current practice” (lecture)?

  9. Discussion vs. Lecture Discussion increased attitude change, when topic relevant Initial attitude .29* Attitude towards nontoxics Discussion vs. lecture .13* .11* Relevance & format Lecture INeffective when relevant Discussion Very effective when relevant

  10. Discussion of relevant information leads to attitude change because students believe others endorse new information .23* Initial attitude .08 Discussion vs. lecture Attitude towards nontoxics .06 33* Relevance & format Perceived group endorsement

  11. ? Discussion Attitude Change PEOPLE BEGIN TO FAVOR NONTOXICS BECAUSE THEY PERCEIVE OTHERS FAVOR NONTOXICS.

  12. Why? • Did lectures and discussion differ in content? • Number of topics covered? • Persuasiveness of coverage?

  13. Behaviors during meeting(from audiotapes) • What did students do that gave others the impression they agreed with the speaker? • Positive participation (enthusiasm, endorsements, questions) • Negative participation (challenges, off-topic remarks)

  14. Private thoughts • Predicted attitude change • not influenced by meetings • Saying vs. thinking

  15. Implications/Problems • Environmental behaviors are social behaviors. • Attitude and behavior change require social support. • Small study; need videotapes

  16. References • Gillilan, S., Werner, C. M., Olson, L., & Adams, D. (1996). Teaching the concept of • PREcycling: A campaign and evaluation. Journal of Environmental Education, 28, 11-18. • Werner, C. M. (2003). Changing homeowners’ use of toxic household products: A transactional approach. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 33-45. • Werner & Adams (2001). Changing homeowners’ behaviors involving toxic household chemicals: A psychological, multilevel approach. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 1, 1-31. [Also available at “www.spssi.org”]. • Werner, C. M., Byerly, S., Sansone, C. (in press). Reducing intentions to use toxic household products through • guided group discussion. In B. Martin & A. Keul (Eds.), Proceedings of IAPS 18, Vienna (July, 2004): IAPS (request from the author carol.werner@psych.utah.edu).

More Related