1 / 53

Landmarks of Moral Formation in Early Childhood

Dan. Landmarks of Moral Formation in Early Childhood. Daniel Lapsley University of Notre Dame. www.nd.edu/~dlapsle1/Lab. Darcia. Infant Morality and the “Received View”. Morality is “declarative” knowledge It is deliberative, explicit, propositional It is wrestling with dilemmas

viola
Download Presentation

Landmarks of Moral Formation in Early Childhood

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Dan Landmarks of Moral Formation in Early Childhood Daniel Lapsley University of Notre Dame www.nd.edu/~dlapsle1/Lab Darcia Conference on Infant and Toddler Mental Health, August 12, 2011

  2. Infant Morality and the “Received View” Morality is “declarative” knowledge It is deliberative, explicit, propositional It is wrestling with dilemmas It is “knowing that”

  3. By age 3 the child’s self is a moral self The child has internalized “rules” of what to do and not to do Displays moral affect Engages in prosocial sharing Regulates conflict between personal needs and social obligations Is governed by internal standards But self and morality develop before onset of reflective self-awareness

  4. Landmarks • Moral Self of Infancy • Empathy and Prosocial Behavior • Toddlers Norms, Standards Concepts • Conscience • Social-Cognitive (“Narrative”) Approach

  5. Robert Emde Origins of the Moral Self? Biologically prepared “motives” Procedural knowledge “In my beginning is my end.” ---T. S. Eliot (East Coker)

  6. Procedural Knowledge Information that underlies skills that need not be represented consciously It is knowing how---but we know more than we can say Intelligent systems can manifest rule-governed behavior without any explicit representation of the rule System 1 vs. System 2

  7. The early self develops procedurally “Existential self” is the first self of infancy (independent existence and agency) Infants’ behavior is coherent, organized, rule-governed, but not always based on acquisition of explicit rules But acquired piecemeal via day-to-day interactions with caregivers Emde (1991)

  8. Early moral development is based on knowledge that is organized procedurally Infants act in accordance with moral rules ----but need not recall them to follow them What is source of this procedural knowledge? Infants are biologically prepared for it! Five “motives” built into our species by evolution And consolidated into an “affective core”

  9. ? But operation, activation and consolidation of basic motives requires a sensitive, responsive infant- care-giver relationship i.e., the affective core requires a context Source: Emde et al (1991)

  10. Contextual Model Emde et al. (1991)

  11. An Early Moral Self Infant rule-learning originates in inborn propensity and in expectablecaregiver relationship experiences Reciprocity Norm Violations Empathy-Sharing Emde et al (1991)

  12. Reciprocity Develops from basic motive of social fittedness Early face-to-face turn-taking with mother an example of internalized rules about reciprocity Rules about how to communicate---engage, maintain and terminate social interactions are operative before language Are internalized as result of pleasurable caregiver experiences and come to form early motives and procedures for social turn-taking Emde et al (1991)

  13. Is this morality? Reciprocity is the “foundation stone” of moral systems Do unto others…

  14. Norm Violations Another feature of basic morality becomes differentiated by end of second year….. Anxiety when internal standards are violated New affective way for “cognitive assimilation” to show itself i.e., for “getting it right” Is this morality? All systems of morality require internalized standards, and “unease” when violated Child’s early moral self has emotional procedures that guide the process by age 3

  15. Empathy Empathy: affective response that stems from apprehension of another’s emotional state & is similar to what the other is feeling or would be expected to feel in given situation; Rules for turn-taking and social communication cultivate empathy Has strong maturational basis Empathy and helping influenced by quality of caregiving Infants produce and comprehend emotional gestures and signal in play episodes; Can use emotional expressions of others to regulate own emotions

  16. Empathy By the first birthday: Is aware that self and others are independent and that minds can be interfaced Inter-subjectivity can be generated by emotional signalling

  17. By 20 months: Infants can label some emotional states By 24 months: can make causal statements (“You sad, Mommy……what daddy do?”) By 24 months children have internalized rules for “do’s” and “don’ts” ---(at least in presence of caregiver) Toddlers are empathically responsive to mood states of others and can reproduce or share in emotion states of others Pre-schoolers can correctly identify emotional reactions of others and its causes

  18. Social referencing Searching for emotional signals during prohibitions Repeated looking before or after prohibited act Or for permitted acts

  19. Is this morality? People who experience another’s emotions and feel concern are expected to help, be altruistic and prosocial; Only prosocial behaviors motivated by empathy-sympathy have moral significance

  20. Empathy and Prosocial Behavior Eisenberg Children under 2 often share toys and give things away By age 2, can verbalize understanding of another’s needs, wants, intentions And comfort a younger sibling, will attempt to alleviate distress by sympathy, offering help These prosocial inclinations are observed throughout early childhood

  21. 1983, Child Development Zahn-Waxler When young children heard infants cry: Children as young as 4-5 displayed signs of emotional arousal Made empathic statements Offered to help (especially when cries were not too intense and baby’s mother was present)

  22. Observed sympathetic concern and prosocial behavior co-occur even at age 2 And co-occur in early childhood When 4-5 year olds witnessed someone in distress, children in all risk groups (low-moderate-hi) showed similar empathic concern and prosocial behavior Moderate and high risk children were less able to remain positively engaged with distress victims

  23. Tomasello After a 3-year old witnessed a puppet destroy another puppet’s belongings: Intervened on behalf of absent victim (verbally protested) Tattled on transgressors Acted pro-socially on behalf of victims of transgression Children as young as 3 years of age actively intervene in third-party moral transgressions….and in defense of moral norms!

  24. Summary • The building blocks of the moral self are evident in infancy • The affective core is organized into procedural moral understanding • Reciprocity • Norm-Violations • Empathy and Prosocial Behavior • Fashioned in sensitive, reciprocal exchanges with emotionally-available caregivers But there is room for improvement!

  25. Present 4-, 6- and 8 year olds a situation where a child commits a moral transgression e.g., steals another’s candy, pushes a child off a swing Then ask: “How would the victimizer feel?” W. Arsenio Nearly all 4- and 6-year olds and most 8-year olds predicted that the victimizer –who gets what he or she wants--- would be happy “Happy Victimizer” The young child has difficulty coordinating material gain of the victimizer with negative consequences to the victim

  26. Landmarks • Moral Self of Infancy • Empathy and Prosocial Behavior • Toddler’s Norms, Standards, Concepts • Conscience • Narrative Self

  27. Sensitivity to Norms and Standards “Ought” Young children develop early normative expectations Towards end of the second year Toddlers become concerned with how things ought to be e.g., names of things they are learning e.g., behavioral routines (inflexible about bedtime rituals) e.g., violations of appearance Children are constructing representations of how things are done and are sensitive to violations of normative expectations

  28. 19-month olds respond negatively and with concern when faced with objects that have been marred, damaged or disfigured J. Kagan Missing buttons, torn pages, broken toys---react with interest, attention and negative evaluation (“It’s yukky!”) --touching the flaw --concern about who was responsible Interpreted as emerging moral sense These damaged objects violate implicit norms of wholeness that parents enforce through sanctions on breaking or damaging objects But perhaps not an emerging moral sense

  29. Is the sensitivity to norms specific to “wrong-doing” (i.e., broken or damaged) Or whether children respond to anything that is simply atypical (e.g., being the wrong color) R. Thompson Compared toddlers response to toy different from the norm in several ways: Some were broken or damaged (teddy bear with one eye missing) Others were functionally impaired without being broken (e.g., teddy bear without stuffing) Some functional and intact but abnormal (e.g., teddy bear with psychedelic colors with wings)

  30. Courtesy of Ross Thompson

  31. Toddlers 14 to 23 months Regardless of age---young children showed no differential responding to the objects implying wrong-doing Instead, they responded with interest, affect and attention to all forms of atypicality–damaged, functionally impaired or abnormal Rather than an emerging moral sense---toddlers’ reaction to broken toys and disfigured objects a more general sensitivity to events different from conventional norms This sensitivity becomes enlisted into an early moral sensibility as children come to learn that broken and marred objects are also disapproved Here---what is atypical is interesting not only because it violates a norm, but is also forbidden Thompson (2009)

  32. Toddler’s Moral Concepts Toddler’s moral judgments nuanced by their understanding of different domains of rules Moral v. Conventional judgments can be distinguished along several criteria (alterability, contingency, generality, seriousness) 3.5 year olds distinguish moral and conventional rules on all criteria; 3 year olds distinguish only generality 2 year olds did not distinguish on any criteria

  33. J. Smetana 24 and 36 month old children Videotaped in two 45-minute sessions at home With mother alone With mother and peers Moral transgressions more frequent in peer interactions Conventional transgressions more frequent with mother alone Mothers response to conventional violations focuses on social order or social regulation Response to moral transgressions focuses transgressor on consequences of actions on rights or welfare of victim

  34. Landmarks • Moral Self of Infancy • Empathy and Prosocial Behavior • Toddler’s Norms, Standards, Concepts • Conscience • Narrative Self

  35. Conscience Inner guiding system responsible for gradual emergence and maintenance of self-regulation Inner self-regulatory system consisting of moral emotions, conduct, cognitions Grazyna Kochanska Conscience influence how children construct and act consistently with generalizable internal standards of conduct Range of individual differences Different pathways to conscience Two sources of individual differences: Biologically-based temperament Socialization experiences with early caregivers

  36. Kochanska’s Model Emerging morality begins with quality of parent-child attachment Strong mutually responsive relationship to caregivers orients child to be receptive to parental influence “Mutually-Responsive Orientation” (MRO) MRO characterized by “shared positive affect” Mutually coordinated enjoyable routines (“good times”) “cooperative interpersonal set” Joint willingness of parent and child to initiate and reciprocate relational overtures

  37. Within MRO, and the secure attachment it denotes, that the child is eager to comply with parental expectations and standards “committed compliance” to norms and values of caregivers Which motivates moral internalization and “conscience”

  38. Two Main Components of Early Conscience Rule-compatible, internalized conduct (rule compliance without surveillance) Moral emotion (empathy) Children who comply with rules even without supervision Who feel empathic concern towards others’ distress Who feel discomfort when they commit transgressions Psychosocial competence Positive development

  39. Children who experience a highly responsive relationship with mothers over first 24 months strongly embraced maternal prohibitions And gave evidence of strong self-regulation at pre-school age

  40. Committed Compliance Security of Attachment (MRO) Moral Internalization “Children with a strong history of committed compliance with the parent are likely gradually to come to view themselves as embracing the parent’s values and rules. Such a moral self, in turn, comes to serve as the regulator of future moral conduct and, more generally, of early morality” (p. 340) But children bring something to the interaction….their temperament

  41. Multiple pathways to conscience One parenting style not uniformly more effective irrespective of child’s temperament Children who are “fearful” would profit from gentle, low power-assertive discipline “silken glove” But “fearless” children would require not the “iron hand” but discipline that capitalizes on positive emotions

  42. Overview of Methodology Longitudinal assessment: 25 mos., 38mos., 52 mos., 67 mos. & 80 mos. Two, 2-3 hour laboratory session, one with each parent At 38 months, one home and one lab (with each parent) Child’s internalization of each parent’s rules and empathy towards parents’ distress observed in scripted paradigms at 25mo., 38mo. & 52 mos. Moral self assessed with “puppet interview” Adaptive, competent, prosocial and antisocial behavior rated by parents & teachers

  43. Moral Self Two puppets anchor opposite ends of 31 items The items pertain to dimensions of early conscience (e.g., internalization of rules, empathy, apology, etc) Each item presented with brief scenario, with one puppet endorsing one option and the second puppet the other option (“with equally self-righteous voices”) Puppet 1: “When I break something, I try to hide it so no one finds out.” Puppet 2: “When I break something, I tell someone right away.” Then the child is asked: “What about you? Do you try to hide something that you broke or do you tell someone right away?”

  44. Other Assessments (at 80 mos.) MacArthur Health Behavior Questionnaire (parents & teachers) School engagement Peer relations Prosocial behavior Problem behavior Child Symptom Inventory Opposition defiant items Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (parents) Absences of guilt & empathy Disregard for rules & standards

  45. Were competent, engaged, prosocial with few antisocial behavioral problems at early school age Children who as toddlers & preschoolers had strong history of internalized “out-of-sight” compliance with parents’ rules Strong history of empathic responding at toddlers/preschool Psychosocial competence at early school age What mechanism accounts for this beneficial effect? The Moral Self Children’s moral self robustly predicted future competent behavior Children at 67 mos. who were “highly moral” were rated at 80 mos. as highly competent, prosocial and having few antisocial problems

  46. Fig. 2 Kochanska et al (2010)

  47. Fig. 3 Kochanska et al. (2010)

  48. How does the moral self execute its inner guidance role? Mechanisms not completely clear Kochanka suggests avoidance of cognitive dissonance anticipation of guilty feelings, automatic regulation due to high accessibility of moral schemas

  49. “In the end is my beginning” ---T.S. Eliot (East Coker) Moral Self of Infancy Conscience “In my beginning is my end.” ---T. S. Eliot (East Coker)

More Related