1 / 38

Andrej A. Kibrik Olga B. Markus

Andrej A. Kibrik Olga B. Markus. Dependent clauses in Upper Kuskokwim Athabaskan. Athabaskan Languages Conferenc e Berkeley, July 2009. Basic information about Upper Kuskokwim Athabaskan (UKA). About 30 speakers left out of the population of about 200

triage
Download Presentation

Andrej A. Kibrik Olga B. Markus

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Andrej A. KibrikOlga B. Markus Dependent clauses in Upper Kuskokwim Athabaskan Athabaskan Languages Conference Berkeley, July 2009

  2. Basic information about Upper Kuskokwim Athabaskan (UKA) • About 30 speakers left out of the population of about 200 • Most speakers reside in the village of Nikolai • Actual use of UKA – in two or three households • Prior work – Collins and Petruska 1979 • Kibrik’s field trips in 1997 and 2001

  3. Welcome to Nikolai

  4. Data • Natural discourse recordings (transcribed) • Folk stories • Personal stories • Conversation (pre-arranged) • Interview at school • In all – 3 hours 20 minutes of talk

  5. Lena Petruska, the oldest speaker

  6. Quantitative data: an overview • 750 clauses in the data set • Independent clauses – 86.1% • Dependent clauses – 13.9% • Complement clauses – 9.8% • Quotative clauses – 7.5% • Adverbial clauses – 3.7% • Relative clauses – 0.4%

  7. Independent clauses • The strongly preferred clause type • Simple clause concatenation often appears even in case of clear adverbial meaning • Always finite: no analog of converbal forms Effect – Cause: (1) ‘I did not take the dogs to the upriver portage (because) the grass was too tall, and <…>’

  8. Complement clauses • Noonan 1985/2007 – a classification of complement taking predicates: • Utterance predicates • Propositional attitude predicates • Pretence predicates • Commentative predicates • Predicates of knowledge • Predicates of fearing • Desiderative predicates • Manipulative predicates • Modal predicates • Achievement predicates • Phasal predicates • Immediate perception predicates • Negative predicates • Conjunctive predicates • Attested • Unattested • Not expectable

  9. Complement clauses • Matrix predicates attested in the UKA data, in the order of decreasing frequency • say, tell • be • become • used to • want • seem • think • hear • see • be true • learn • forget • pretend • feel

  10. Complement clauses: quotative • Quotative clauses: by far the most frequent class among complement clauses, and in fact among all dependent clauses • All instances of quotation are direct quotations (2) ‘ “Feed them [the dogs]”, he [the giant] told him [the brother]’ OR ‘He [the giant] told him [the brother] to feed them [the dogs]’

  11. Complement clauses: quotative • Two clauses form a prosodic complex: (3) ‘I thought that I would set traps around here instead’

  12. Complement clauses (frequent) • ‘be’ (4) ‘The fact is that is baptized our way’ • ‘become’ (5) ‘Your children will become such that they steal things’ • ‘used to’ (6) ‘What was it that they mostly used to hunt for?’

  13. Complement clauses (mid-frequent) • ‘seem’ (7) ‘It seems he is listening to us’ • ‘want’ (8) ‘Do you want that he brews tea for you?’

  14. Complementizer ts’eŒ • Attested with the matrix verbs: • ‘want’ • ‘learn’ • ‘forget’ • ‘not know’

  15. Exceptional head-dependent word order (9) ‘He heard that the dogs were panting out there’ OR ‘He heard: the dogs were panting out there’

  16. Interposition • Not attested in natural discourse, but elicited: (10) ‘John told him that he would come’

  17. Adverbial clauses

  18. Adverbial clauses: temporal • Preposition with respect to the main clause (11) ‘Both when you start eating and when you go to bed, always pray’

  19. Adverbial clause: causal • Postposition with respect to the main clause (12) ‘I did not sleep because he was snoring’

  20. Relative clause • Extremely rare • Almost no examples of noun-headed relative clauses (13) ‘The one whom they call Big Foot took her, that one’ • Elicited: (14) ‘I saw a long boat’

  21. Unusually complex construction (15) ‘ “When you grow up, your children will become such that they steal things”, she told me instead’

  22. Impressionistic conclusions • Extreme preference for • independent clauses • clause chaining • finite verbs • Very little interclausal syntax • The only statistically salient type of dependent clause: quotative • Relatively frequent are only those dependent clause types that are lexically predetermined, that is, complements • More discourse-oriented dependent clause types, including adverbial and relative clauses, are very rare, even when the appropriate grammatical equipment is available

  23. However, compare with a very different language

  24. Reassessment • Scarcity of dependent clauses in UKA is due primarily to universal factors than to specifics of the given language • The impression of scarcity stems from our intuitive judgments based on written and normative language

  25. Positive conclusions • Strong dispreference for relative clauses • Absence of non-finite forms • in complement clauses (cf. infinitives or deverbal nouns or other non-finite forms in many languages) • Navajo -ígíí is used in some complements • in adverbial clauses (cf. converbal forms in many languages) • Navajo –go is massively used in “cosubordination” • Syntax of complex constructions is maximally simple • Real specialty of Athabaskan lies in morphology, not in syntax

More Related