1 / 33

The Limits of Expertise: The Misunderstood Role of Pilot Error in Airline Accidents

The Limits of Expertise: The Misunderstood Role of Pilot Error in Airline Accidents. Key Dismukes NASA Ames Research Center Ben Berman and Loukia Loukopoulos San Jose State University Foundation/NASA Ames Research Center ASPA/ICAO Regional Seminar 8-9 March 2005.

sund
Download Presentation

The Limits of Expertise: The Misunderstood Role of Pilot Error in Airline Accidents

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. The Limits of Expertise:The Misunderstood Role of Pilot Error in Airline Accidents Key Dismukes NASA Ames Research Center Ben Berman and Loukia Loukopoulos San Jose State University Foundation/NASA Ames Research Center ASPA/ICAO Regional Seminar 8-9 March 2005

  2. Most Airline Accidents Attributed to Crew Error What does this mean? • Why do highly skilled pilots make fatal errors? • How should we think about the role of errors in accidents? Draw upon cognitive science research on skilled performance of human operators

  3. Approach • Reviewed NTSB reports of the 19 U.S. airline accidents between 1991-2000 attributed primarily to crew error • Asked: Why might any airline crew in situation of accident crew – knowing only what they knew – be vulnerable? • Can never know with certainty why accident crew made specific errors but can determine why the population of pilots is vulnerable • Considers variability of expert performance as function of interplay of multiple factors

  4. Hindsight Bias • Knowing the outcome of an accident flight reveals what crew should have done differently • Accident crew does not know the outcome • They respond to situation as they perceive it at the moment • Principle of “local rationality”: experts do what seems reasonable, given what they know at the moment and the limits of human information processing • Errors are not de facto evidence of lack of skill or lack of conscientiousness

  5. Two Fallacies About Human Error Myth: Experts who make errors performing a familiar task reveal lack of skill, vigilance, or conscientiousness Fact: Skill, vigilance, and conscientiousness are essential but not sufficient to prevent error Myth: If experts can normally perform a task without difficulty, they should always be able to perform that task correctly Fact: Experts periodically make errors as consequence of subtle variations in task demands, information available, and cognitive processing

  6. Immediate demands of situation & tasks being performed • Social/Organizational • Influences • Formal procedures & • policies • Explicit goals & rewards • Implicit goals & rewards • Actual “norms” for line operations Training, experience, & personal goals Human cognition characteristics & limitations Crew responses to situation

  7. A Truism • No one thing “causes” accidents • Confluence of multiple events, actions taken or not taken, and environmental factors

  8. Confluence of Factors in a CFIT Accident Approach controller failed to update altimeter setting Training & Standardization issues? Weather conditions Non-precision approach ≥ 250 foot terrain clearance Rapid change in barometric pressure Strong crosswind Tower window broke Autopilot would not hold PF used Altitude Hold to capture MDA PM used non-standard callouts to alert PF Are most pilots aware of this? Tower closed PF selected Heading Select Altimeter update not available Altitude Hold may allow altitude sag 130 feet in turbulence Airline’s use of QFE altimetry Additional workload ? Increased vulnerability to error ? Crew error (70 feet) in altimeter setting 170 foot error in altimeter reading Aircraft struck trees 310 feet below MDA

  9. Chance Combination of Contributing Factors • Airline accidents are extremely rare in modern operations • Countermeasures in place for single-point failures of equipment or human performance • Occasionally accidents slip through multiple defenses because of chance combination of multiple factors • Number of possible combinations/permutations of factors is vast • Difficult to devise countermeasures • Vague advice to pilots to “break the accident chain” is not helpful

  10. Each Accident Has Unique Surface Features and Combinations of Factors • Countermeasures to surface features of past accidents will not prevent future accidents • Must examine deep structure of accidents to find common factors

  11. Six Overlapping Clusters of Error Patterns • Inadvertent slips and oversights while performing highly practiced tasks under normal conditions • Inadvertent slips and oversights while performing highly practiced tasks under challenging conditions • Inadequate execution of non-normal procedures under challenging conditions • Inadequate response to rare situations for which pilots are not trained • Judgment in ambiguous situations • Deviation from explicit guidance or SOP

  12. 1) Inadvertent Slips/Oversights in Practiced Tasks under Normal Conditions • Examples: • Omitting procedural step or checklist item • Remembering altimeter setting incorrectly • Misjudging landing flare • Identical to errors pilots frequently report to ASRS and ASAP and errors observed in LOSA • Commonplace error had to combine with multiple other factors to result in accident

  13. 2) Inadvertent Slips/Oversights in Practiced Tasks under Challenging Conditions • Probability of commonplace errors goes up with workload, time pressure, fatigue and stress • Snowball effects: events/decisions/actions increase workload, time pressure, and stress downstream, increasing chance of more problems and errors

  14. 3) Inadequate Execution of Non-normal Procedures under Challenging Conditions • Failure to recover from spiral dive, stall, or windshear • Veridian study suggests existing training not sufficient • Surprise, confusion, and stress may impede correct diagnosis of upset and timely execution of appropriate procedure

  15. 4) Inadequate Response to Rare Situations for which Pilots are not Trained • Examples: • False stick shaker activation just after rotation • Oversensitive autopilot drove aircraft down at Decision Height • Anomalous airspeed indications past rotation speed • Uncommanded autothrottle disconnect with non-salient annunciation • Surprise, confusion, stress, and time pressure play a role • No data on what percentage of airline pilots would respond adequately in these situations

  16. 5) Judgment and Decision-making in Ambiguous Situations • Examples: • Continuing approach in vicinity of thunderstorms • Not de-icing or not repeating de-icing • No algorithm to calculate when to break off approach; company guidance usually generic • Crew must integrate incomplete and fragmentary information and make best judgment • If guess wrong, crew error is found to be “cause” • Accident crew judgment & decision-making may not differ from non-accident crews in similar situations: • Lincoln Lab study: Penetration of storm cells on approach not uncommon • Other flights may have landed or taken off without difficulty a minute or two before accident flight • Questions: • What are actual industry norms for these operations? • Sufficient guidance for crews to balance competing goals? • Implicitly tolerate/encourage less conservative behavior as long as crews get by with it?

  17. 6) Deviation from Explicit Guidance or SOP • Example: Attempting to land from unstabilized approach resulting from slam-dunk approach • Simple willful violation or more complex issue? • Are stabilized approach criteria published/trained as guidance or absolute bottom lines? • What are norms in company and the industry? • Pilots may not realize that struggling to stabilize approach before touchdown imposes such workload that they cannot evaluate whether landing will work out

  18. Cross-Cutting Factors Contributing to Crew Errors

  19. Situations Requiring Rapid Response • Nearly 2/3 of 19 accidents • Examples: upset attitudes, false stick shaker activation after rotation, anomalous airspeed indications at rotation, autopilot-induced oscillation at Decision Height, pilot-induced oscillation during flare • Very rare occurrences, but high risk • Surprise is a factor • Inadequate time to think through situation • automatic response required

  20. Challenges of Managing Concurrent Tasks • A factor in great majority of these accidents • Workload quite high in some accidents: • Crews became so overloaded they failed to recognize situation was getting out of control • Crews may fail to notice subtle cues and integrate information from multiple sources • Crews may be forced into reactive mode rather than strategic mode • Monitoring and cross-checking suffer

  21. Challenges of Managing Concurrent Tasks (continued) • In many accidents adequate time was available for all tasks, however: • Especially vulnerable to error when switching attention among tasks, interrupted, distracted, or forced to defer tasks out of normal sequence • Vulnerability inherent in basic cognitive processes • Can attend to only one distinct task at a given instant • Once attention is diverted from a task do not always remember to resume task if not prompted • Better monitoring can help prevent/catch errors • But monitoring is itself a concurrent task and vulnerable to the same factors that produce errors

  22. Equipment Failures and Design Flaws • Occurred in 2/3 of these accidents • Some equipment failures/design flaws precipitated chain of events Example: false stick shaker after rotation • Some equipment failures/design flaws undermined efforts of crew to respond Example: stick shaker failed to activate when aircraft approached stall

  23. Misleading or Missing Cues Normally Present • False stick shaker is misleading cue • Hard to sort out under time pressure, high workload, and stress • Failure of stick shaker removes expected cue • Crew errors also generate misleading cues or remove normal cues • e.g., premature Vr callout omission of speed and vertical speed callouts

  24. Plan Continuation Bias • Unconscious cognitive bias to continue original plan in spite of changing conditions • Appears stronger as one nears completion of activity (e.g., approach to landing) • Why are crews reluctant to go-around? • Bias may prevent noticing subtle cues indicating original conditions have changed • May combine with other cognitive biases: • Frequency sampling bias (“It’s always worked before”) • Reactive responding is easier than proactive thinking

  25. Stress • Hard to evaluate extent, but stress is normal physiological/behavioral response to threat • Acute stress hampers performance • Narrows attention (“tunneling”) • Reduces working memory capacity • Combination of surprise, stress, time pressure, and concurrent task demands can be lethal setup • Beginning NASA research project on effects of stress on crew performance

  26. Shortcomings in Training and/or Guidance • >1/3 of accidents • Inadequate guidance to pilots about known problems (e.g. high sensitivity of wings without leading edge devices to minute amounts of frost) • Upset attitude recovery training • How to deal with fact that not possible to train for every possible situation?

  27. Social/Organizational Issues • Actual norms may deviate from Flight Operations Manuals • Little data available on extent to which accident crews’ actions are typical/atypical • Competing pressures not often acknowledged • e.g., on-time performance vs. conservative response to ambiguous situations • Pilots may not be consciously aware of influence of internalized competing goals

  28. Implications and Countermeasures • Focus on deep structure, not superficial manifestations • Most accidents are systems accidents • Unrealistic to expect human operators to never make an error • Unrealistic to think can automate humans or error out of the system • Design overall operating system for resilience to equipment failure, unexpected events, uncertainty, and human error

  29. Implications and Countermeasures (continued) • Need better info on how airspace system typically operates and how crews respond • e.g., frequency/site of slam-dunk clearances, last-minute runway changes, unstabilized approaches • FOQA and LOSA are sources of information • NASA research for next generation FOQA: Aviation Performance Measurement System (APMS) • Dr. Tom Chidester: > 1% of 16,000 flights: high energy arrivals unstabilized approaches landing exceedances • Must find ways to share FOQA and LOSA data industry-wide to develop comprehensive picture of system vulnerabilities

  30. Implications and Countermeasures (continued) • When FOQA and LOSA uncover deviations from ideal, must find why e.g., must identify forces discouraging crews from abandoning unstabilized approaches • Concern for on-time performance and fuel costs? • Viewed as lack of skill? • Fear of recrimination? • Fail to recognize logic for unstabilized approach criteria?

  31. Implications and Countermeasures (continued) • Pilots, airline managers, instructors, designers of equipment and procedures must be well educated about human cognitive characteristics and limitations • Interaction of cognitive processes with task demands drives vulnerability to error • Airlines should periodically review normal and non-normal procedures for vulnerability to error e.g., checklists are vulnerable to “looking without seeing”, interruptions, and deferred items Can reduce vulnerability: • Execute checklists in slow, deliberate manner, pointing and touching • Anchor checklist initiation to salient events (e.g., top of descent) • Treat interruptions and deferred items as red flags and create salient reminder cues

  32. Implications and Countermeasures (continued) • Repetitiousness can lead to: Briefings becoming mindless recitation and crews becoming reactive rather than proactive Solution: train pilots to use briefings as tool to look ahead, question situation, identify threats, and prepare options • Beef up training: • Upset attitude recovery with realistic scenarios • Monitoring • Acknowledge inherent trade-offs between safety and system efficiency • Include all parties in analysis of trade-offs • Make policy decisions explicit

  33. More information on NASA Human Factors Research: http://human-factors.arc.nasa.gov/ihs/flightcognition/

More Related