sample design for group randomized trials l.
Download
Skip this Video
Loading SlideShow in 5 Seconds..
Sample Design for Group-Randomized Trials PowerPoint Presentation
Download Presentation
Sample Design for Group-Randomized Trials

Loading in 2 Seconds...

play fullscreen
1 / 61

Sample Design for Group-Randomized Trials - PowerPoint PPT Presentation


  • 120 Views
  • Uploaded on

Sample Design for Group-Randomized Trials. Howard S. Bloom Chief Social Scientist MDRC Prepared for the IES/NCER Summer Research Training Institute held at Northwestern University on July 27, 2010. Today we will examine . Sample size determinants Precision requirements Sample allocation

loader
I am the owner, or an agent authorized to act on behalf of the owner, of the copyrighted work described.
capcha
Download Presentation

PowerPoint Slideshow about 'Sample Design for Group-Randomized Trials' - raeanne


An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation

Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author.While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server.


- - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - E N D - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -
Presentation Transcript
sample design for group randomized trials

Sample Design for Group-Randomized Trials

Howard S. Bloom

Chief Social Scientist

MDRC

Prepared for the IES/NCER Summer Research Training Institute held at Northwestern University on July 27, 2010.

today we will examine
Today we will examine
  • Sample size determinants
  • Precision requirements
  • Sample allocation
  • Covariate adjustments
  • Matching and blocking
  • Subgroup analyses
  • Generalizing findings for sites and blocks
  • Using two-level data for three-level situations
part i

Part I:

The Basics

statistical properties of group randomized impact estimators
Statistical properties of group-randomized impact estimators

Unbiased estimates

Yij = a+B0Tj+ej+eij

E(b0) = B0

Less precise estimates

VAR(eij) = s2

VAR(ej) = t2

r = t2/(t2+s2)

design effect for a given total number of individuals
Design Effect(for a given total number of individuals)

______________________________________

IntraclassIndividuals per Group (n)

Correlation (r) 10 50 500

  • 0.01 1.04 1.22 2.48
  • 0.05 1.20 1.86 5.09
  • 0.10 1.38 2.43 7.13
  • _____________________________________
sample design parameters
Sample design parameters
  • Number of randomized groups (J)
  • Number of individuals per randomized group (n)
  • Proportion of groups randomized to program status (P)
reporting precision
Reporting precision
  • A minimum detectable effect (MDE) is the smallest true effect that has a “good chance” of being found to be statistically significant.
  • We typically define an MDE as the smallest true effect that has 80 percent power for a two-tailed test of statistical significance at the 0.05 level.
  • An MDE is reported in natural units whereas a minimum detectable effect size (MDES) is reported in units of standard deviations
minimum detectable effect sizes for a group randomized design with r 0 05 and no covariates
Minimum Detectable Effect SizesFor a Group-Randomized Design with r = 0.05 and no Covariates

___________________________________

Randomized Individuals per Group (n)

Groups (J) 10 50 500

10 0.77 0.53 0.46

20 0.50 0.35 0.30

40 0.35 0.24 0.21

120 0.20 0.14 0.12

___________________________________

implications for sample design
Implications for sample design
  • It is extremely important to randomize an adequate number of groups.
  • It is often far less important how many individuals per group you have.
part ii

Part II

Determining required precision

when assessing how much precision is needed
When assessing how much precision is needed:

Always ask “relative to what?”

  • Program benefits
  • Program costs
  • Existing outcome differences
  • Past program performance
effect size gospel according to cohen and lipsey
Effect Size Gospel According to Cohen and Lipsey

Cohen Lipsey

(speculative) (empirical)

_______________________________________________

Small = 0.2s Small = 0.15s

Medium = 0.5s Medium = 0.45s

Large = 0.8s Large = 0.90s

five year impacts of the tennessee class size experiment
Five-year impacts of the Tennessee class-size experiment

Treatment:

13-17 versus 22-26 students per class

Effect sizes:

0.11s to 0.22s for reading and math

Findings are summarized from Nye, Barbara, Larry V. Hedges and Spyros Konstantopoulos (1999) “The Long-Term Effects of Small Classes: A Five-Year Follow-up of the Tennessee Class Size Experiment,” Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, Vol. 21, No. 2: 127-142.

annual reading and math growth

Annual reading and math growth

Reading Math

Grade Growth Growth

Transition Effect Size Effect Size

----------------------------------------------------------------

K - 1 1.52 1.14

1 - 2 0.97 1.03

2 - 3 0.60 0.89

3 - 4 0.36 0.52

4 - 5 0.40 0.56

5 - 6 0.32 0.41

6 - 7 0.23 0.30

7 - 8 0.26 0.32

8 - 9 0.24 0.22

9 - 10 0.19 0.25

10 - 11 0.19 0.14

11 - 12 0.06 0.01

-------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------

Based on work in progress using documentation on the national norming samples for the CAT5, SAT9, Terra Nova CTBS, Gates MacGinitie (for reading only), MAT8, Terra Nova CAT, and SAT10. 95% confidence intervals range in reading from +/- .03 to .15 and in math from +/- .03 to .22

performance gap between average 50 th percentile and weak 10 th percentile schools
Performance gap between “average” (50th percentile) and “weak” (10th percentile) schools

Source: District I outcomes are based on ITBS scaled scores, District II on SAT 9 scaled scores, District III on MAT NCE scores, and District IV on SAT 8 NCE scores.

demographic performance gap in reading and math main naep scores
Demographic performance gap in reading and math: Main NAEP scores

Source: U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Statistics, National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP), 2002 Reading Assessment and 2000 Mathematics Assessment.

part iii

Part III

The ABCs of Sample Allocation

sample allocation alternatives
Sample allocation alternatives

Balanced allocation

  • maximizes precision for a given sample size;
  • maximizes robustness to distributional assumptions.

Unbalanced allocation

  • precision erodes slowly with imbalance for a given sample size
  • imbalance can facilitate a larger sample
  • Imbalance can facilitate randomization
variance relationships for the program and control groups
Variance relationships for the program and control groups
  • Equal variances: when the program does not affect the outcome variance.
  • Unequal variances: when the program does affect the outcome variance.
minimum detectable effect size for sample allocations given equal variances
Minimum Detectable Effect Size For Sample Allocations Given Equal Variances

AllocationExample* Ratio to Balanced

Allocation

0.5/0.5 0.54s 1.00

0.6/0.4 0.55s 1.02

0.7/0.3 0.59s 1.09

0.8/0.2 0.68s 1.25

0.9/0.1 0.91s 1.67

________________________________________

* Example is for n = 20, J = 10, r = 0.05, a one-tail hypothesis test and no covariates.

implications continued
Implications Continued

The estimated standard error is unbiased

  • When the allocation is balanced
  • When the variances are equal

The estimated standard error is biased upward

  • When the larger sample has the larger variance

The estimated standard error is biased downward

  • When the larger sample has the smaller variance
interim conclusions
Interim Conclusions
  • Don’t use the equal variance assumption for an unbalanced allocation with many degrees of freedom.
  • Use a balanced allocation when there are few degrees of freedom.
references
References

Gail, Mitchell H., Steven D. Mark, Raymond J. Carroll, Sylvan B. Green and David Pee (1996) “On Design Considerations and Randomization-Based Inferences for Community Intervention Trials,” Statistics in Medicine 15: 1069 – 1092.

Bryk, Anthony S. and Stephen W. Raudenbush (1988) “Heterogeneity of Variance in Experimental Studies: A Challenge to Conventional Interpretations,” Psychological Bulletin, 104(3): 396 – 404.

part iv

Part IV

Using Covariates to Reduce

Sample Size

basic ideas
Basic ideas
  • Goal: Reduce the number of clusters randomized
  • Approach: Reduce the standard error of the impact estimator by controlling for baseline covariates
  • Alternative Covariates
    • Individual-level
    • Cluster-level
    • Pretests
    • Other characteristics
impact estimation with a covariate
Impact Estimation with a Covariate

yij = the outcome for student i from school j

Tj = 1 for treatment schools and 0 for control schools

Xj = a covariate for school j

xij = a covariate for student i from school j

ej = a random error term for school j

eij = a random error term for student i from school j

minimum detectable effect size with a covariate
Minimum Detectable Effect Size with a Covariate

MDES = minimum detectable effect size

MJ-K = a degrees-of-freedom multiplier1

J = the total number of schools randomized

n = the number of students in a grade per school

P = the proportion of schools randomized to treatment

  • = the unconditional intraclass correlation (without a covariate)

R12 = the proportion of variance across individuals within schools (at level 1) predicted by the covariate

R22 = the proportion of variance across schools (at level 2) predicted by the covariate

1 For 20 or more degrees of freedom MJ-K equals 2.8 for a two-tail test and 2.5 for a one-tail test with statistical power of 0.80 and statistical significance of 0.05

questions addressed empirically about the predictive power of covariates
Questions Addressed Empirically about the Predictive Power of Covariates
  • School-level vs. student-level pretests
  • Earlier vs. later follow-up years
  • Reading vs. math
  • Elementary vs. middle vs. high school
  • All schools vs. low-income schools vs. low-performing schools
empirical analysis
Empirical Analysis
  • Estimate r, R22 and R12 from data on thousands of students from hundreds of schools, during multiple years at five urban school districts
  • Summarize these estimates for reading and math in grades 3, 5, 8 and 10
  • Compute implications for minimum detectable effect sizes
estimated parameters for reading with a school level pretest lagged one year
Estimated Parameters for Reading with a School-level Pretest Lagged One Year

___________________________________________________________________

School District

___________________________________________________________

A B C D E

___________________________________________________________________

Grade 3

r 0.20 0.15 0.19 0.22 0.16

R22 0.31 0.77 0.74 0.51 0.75

Grade 5

r 0.25 0.15 0.20 NA 0.12

R22 0.33 0.50 0.81 NA 0.70

Grade 8

r 0.18 NA 0.23 NA NA

R22 0.77 NA 0.91 NA NA

Grade 10

r 0.15 NA 0.29 NA NA

R22 0.93 NA 0.95 NA NA

____________________________________________________________________

slide35
Minimum Detectable Effect Sizes for Reading with a School-Level Pretest (Y-1) or a Student-Level Pretest (y-1) Lagged One Year

________________________________________________________

Grade 3 Grade 5 Grade 8 Grade 10

________________________________________________________

20 schools randomized

No covariate 0.57 0.56 0.61 0.62

Y-1 0.37 0.38 0.24 0.16

y-1 0.38 0.40 0.28 0.15

40 schools randomized

No covariate 0.39 0.38 0.42 0.42

Y-1 0.26 0.26 0.17 0.11

y-1 0.26 0.27 0.19 0.10

60 schools randomized

No covariate 0.32 0.31 0.34 0.34

Y-1 0.21 0.21 0.13 0.09

y-1 0.21 0.22 0.15 0.08

________________________________________________________

key findings
Key Findings
  • Using a pretest improves precision dramatically.
  • This improvement increases appreciably from elementary school to middle school to high school because R22 increases.
  • School-level pretests produce as much precision as do student-level pretests.
  • The effect of a pretest declines somewhat as the time between it and the post-test increases.
  • Adding a second pretest increases precision slightly.
  • Using a pretest for a different subject increases precision substantially.
  • Narrowing the sample to schools that are similar to each other does not improve precision beyond that achieved by a pretest.
source
Source

Bloom, Howard S., Lashawn Richburg-Hayes and Alison Rebeck Black (2007) “Using Covariates to Improve Precision for Studies that Randomize Schools to Evaluate Educational Interventions”Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 29(1): 30 – 59.

part v the putative power of pairing

Part VThe Putative Power of Pairing

A Tail of Two Tradeoffs

(“It was the best of techniques. It was the worst of techniques.”

Who the dickens said that?)

pairing
Pairing

Why match pairs?

  • for face validity
  • for precision

How to match pairs?

  • rank order clusters by covariate
  • pair clusters in rank-ordered list
  • randomize clusters in each pair
when to pair
When to pair?
  • When the gain in predictive power outweighs the loss of degrees of freedom
  • Degrees of freedom
    • J - 2 without pairing
    • J/2 - 1 with pairing
deriving the minimum required predictive power of pairing
Deriving the Minimum Required Predictive Power of Pairing

Without pairing

With pairing

Breakeven R2

the minimum required predictive power of pairing
The Minimum Required Predictive Power of Pairing

Randomized Required Predictive

Clusters (J) Power (R min2)*

6 0.52

8 0.35

10 0.26

20 0.11

30 0.07

*For a two-tail test.

a few key points about blocking
A few key points about blocking
  • Blocking for face validity vs. blocking for precision
  • Treating blocks as fixed effects vs.random effects
  • Defining blocks using baseline information
part vi

Part VI

Subgroup Analyses #1:

When to Emphasize Them

confirmatory vs exploratory findings
Confirmatory vs. Exploratory Findings
  • Confirmatory: Draw conclusions about the program’s effectiveness if results are
      • Consistent with theory and contextual factors
      • Statistically significant and large
      • And subgroup was pre-specified
  • Exploratory: Develop hypotheses for further study
pre specification
Pre-specification
  • Before the analysis, state that conclusions about the program will be based in part on findings for this set of subgroups
  • Pre-specification can be based on
    • Theory
    • Prior evidence
    • Policy relevance
statistical significance
Statistical significance
  • When should we discuss subgroup findings?
  • Depends on
    • Whether significant differences in impacts across subgroups
    • Might depend on whether impacts for the full sample are statistically significant
part vii

Part VII

Subgroup Analyses #2:

Creating Subgroups

defining features
Defining Features
  • Creating subgroups in terms of:
    • Program characteristics
    • Randomized group characteristics
    • Individual characteristics
defining subgroups by program characteristics
Defining Subgroups by Program Characteristics
  • Based only on program features that were randomized
  • Thus one cannot use implementation quality
defining subgroups by characteristics of randomized groups
Defining Subgroups by Characteristics Of Randomized Groups
  • Types of impacts
    • Net impacts
    • Differential impacts
  • Internal validity
    • only use pre-existing characteristics
  • Precision
    • Net impact estimates are limited by reduced number of randomized groups
    • Differential impact estimates are triply limited (and often need four times as many randomized groups)
defining subgroups by characteristics of individuals
Defining Subgroups by Characteristics of Individuals
  • Types of impacts
    • Net impacts
    • Differential impacts
  • Internal validity
    • Only use pre-existing characteristics
    • Only use subgroups with sample members from all randomized groups
  • Precision
    • For net impacts: can be almost as good as for full sample
    • For differential impacts: can be even better than for full sample
part viii

Part VIII

Generalizing Results from

Multiple Sites and Blocks

fixed vs random effects inference a vexing issue
Fixed vs. Random Effects Inference:A Vexing Issue
  • Known vs. unknown populations
  • Broader vs. narrower inferences
  • Weaker vs. stronger precision
  • Few vs. many sites or blocks
weighting sites and blocks
Weighting Sites and Blocks
  • Implicitly through a pooled regression
  • Explicitly based on
    • Number of schools
    • Number of students
  • Explicitly based on precision
    • Fixed effects
    • Random effects
  • Bottom line: the question addressed is what counts
part ix

Part IX

Using Two-Level Data for Three-Level Situations

the issue
The Issue
  • General Question: What happens when you design a study with randomized groups that comprise three levels based on data which do not account explicitly for the middle level?
  • Specific Example: What happens when you design a study that randomizes schools (with students clustered in classrooms in schools) based on data for students clustered in schools?
further references
Further References

Bloom, Howard S. (2005) “Randomizing Groups to Evaluate Place-Based Programs,” in Howard S. Bloom, editor, Learning More From Social Experiments: Evolving Analytic Approaches (New York: Russell Sage Foundation).

Bloom, Howard S., Lashawn Richburg-Hayes and Alison Rebeck Black (2005) “Using Covariates to Improve Precision: Empirical Guidance for Studies that Randomize Schools to Measure the Impacts of Educational Interventions” (New York: MDRC).

Donner, Allan and Neil Klar (2000) Cluster Randomization Trials in Health Research (London: Arnold).

Hedges, Larry V. and Eric C. Hedberg (2006) “Intraclass Correlation Values for Planning Group Randomized Trials in Education” (Chicago: Northwestern University).

Murray, David M. (1998) Design and Analysis of Group-Randomized Trials (New York: Oxford University Press).

Raudenbush, Stephen W., Andres Martinez and Jessaca Spybrook (2005) “Strategies for Improving Precision in Group-Randomized Experiments” (University of Chicago).

Raudenbush, Stephen W. (1997) “Statistical Analysis and Optimal Design for Cluster Randomized Trials” Psychological Methods, 2(2): 173 – 185.

Schochet, Peter Z. (2005) “Statistical Power for Random Assignment Evaluations of Education Programs,” (Princeton, NJ: Mathematica Policy Research).