1 / 45

Prenatal Nutrition and IQ: A causal analysis using a Mendelian randomization approach

Prenatal Nutrition and IQ: A causal analysis using a Mendelian randomization approach. Sarah Lewis. A few earlier studies. Common problems in observational studies. Measurement error Reporting/interviewer bias Reverse causation CONFOUNDING. Mendelian randomization.

hei
Download Presentation

Prenatal Nutrition and IQ: A causal analysis using a Mendelian randomization approach

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Prenatal Nutrition and IQ: A causal analysis using a Mendelianrandomization approach Sarah Lewis

  2. A few earlier studies

  3. Common problems in observational studies • Measurement error • Reporting/interviewer bias • Reverse causation • CONFOUNDING

  4. Mendelian randomization Comparison of groups of individuals defined by genotype should only differ with respect to the locus under study. With control for population structure, there should be little confounding by any behavioural, socioeconomic or physiological factors.

  5. Mendel’s second law – the law of independentassortment “The behaviour of each pair of differentiating characteristics in hybrid union is independent of the other differences between the two original plants, and, further, the hybrid produces just so many kinds of egg and pollen cells as there are possible constant combination forms” GregorMendel, 1865.

  6. Problems of observational epidemiology can be overcome using genetic variants as proxies • Confounding • Reverse causation • Biological • Due to reporting bias • Measurement error Independent assortment Fixed at conception Can be measured (genotyped)

  7. Clustered environments and randomised genes (93 phenotypes, 23 SNPs) Davey Smith et al. PLoS Medicine 2008

  8. MR Vs RCT

  9. Objective • To determine whether exposure to specific dietary factors in uteroand infancy influences cognition • Hypotheses • Suboptimum levels of nutrients in uterolead to impaired neurodevelopment and cognitive ability • Variation in genes related to nutrient metabolism is associated with neurodevelopment in infancy • Association of maternal genotype with measurements of cognitive ability, independent of child’s genotype, will indicate a role of the prenatal environment

  10. Project design Nutrient exposure IQ at age 8 Maternal genetic variants affecting nutrient levels confounders Offspring genetic variants

  11. Association between diet and cognition can be affected by confounding by lifestyle factors, reverse causality and measurement error • Polymorphisms in genes that metabolise nutrients can be used as proxies for differences in dietary intake and therefore to infer causal relationships between nutrients and cognition without the above problems

  12. ALSPAC (Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children) • Population-based prospective study conducted in Bristol, England to evaluate factors that affect health and development of children • ~ 14,000 pregnant women enrolled between April 1991 and December 1992 • Information on mother and child collected at regular intervals and ongoing • DNA samples available for mothers and children (~10000 each, ~7000 duos)

  13. Population Characteristics

  14. Potential Confounders • Mother: • Age • Education • Social class • Marital status • Parity • Inter-pregnancy interval • Any infection during pregnancy • Housing tenure • Ever smoked • Alcohol consumption • Iron, zinc, calcium, folic acid, vitamins, other supplements during pregnancy

  15. Potential Confounders • Child: • Sex • Age • Gestation • Birth weight • Breastfeeding duration

  16. Confounders continued

  17. Do low levels of vitamin B in utero lead to a lower IQ at age 8?

  18. Only synthesised by microorganisms • Main sources: fish, shellfish, eggs, meat, dairy products • Recommended Daily Amount: 2-3 ug/day • Dietary deficiency rare (vegans at risk) • B12 deficiency: <150 pmol/l • Main functions: red blood cell formation, DNA synthesis, maintenance of healthy nervous system • Transport: 80% bound to transcobalamin I (HC) 20% bound to transcobalamin II (holoTC). holoTC delivers B12 to cells. • Stored in the liver Vitamin B12 facts

  19. Birth defects Spontaneous abortion Pre-eclampsia Prematurity Low birth weight Cardiovascular disease Cognitive deficit Dementia indicators of B12 deficiency modified from Nexo and Hoffmann-Lucke (2011)

  20. Lower cognition tests scores among offspring of mothers with deficient intake of B12 (Mexico; del Rio Garcia et al., 2009) • Children of mothers with low B12 levels performed worse in sustained-attention and working memory tests (India; Bhate et al., 2008) • No association of maternal B12 levels with cognitive performance in children. Although verbal ability scores were higher in children of mothers with low B12 (India; Veena et al., 2010) • Problem: residual confounding? Vitamin B12 status during pregnancyand cognition in children

  21. Observational Study Model 1: Adjusted for offspring sex and age at time of IQ assessment, and maternal energy intake. Model 2: Model 1 + maternal education, social class, age at delivery, parity, any infection in pregnancy, ever smoked, alcohol consumption before and during pregnancy, folate supplementation. Model 3: Model 2 + gestational length and birth weight.

  22. Instrumental variables

  23. Maternal SNPs vs offspring IQ

  24. Conclusions • Genotypes associated with high vitamin B12 levels are associated with higher IQ. • However, the effect of genotype on exposure is small, therefore the power of the study to confidently detect an effect is low. • Replication is needed.

  25. Do low prenatal iron levels affect IQ?

  26. Iron deficiency anemia in pregnant women Source: Worldwide Prevalence of Anemia - WHO report 2008

  27. Effects of iron deficiency in early life • developing brain structures (striatum, hippocampus) • neurotransmitter systems (dopamine, serotonin) • Synaptogenesis • Myelination • different gene and protein profiles in the ID brain

  28. Consequences for the child • recognition memory function • temperament (irritability, lower alertness and soothability) • hand-eye movement • locomotion • comprehension of language • fine motor skills • school performance • long lasting abnormalities, even after iron repletion

  29. Instruments: TF, TMPRSS6, HFE SNPs rs1799945 C/G H63D rs1800562 G/A C282Y serum iron serum transferrin serum ferritin haemoglobin transferrin saturation C & G= iron lowering Kullo et al. (2010) rs3811647 G/A serum transferrin serum ferritin transferrin saturation A = iron lowering Benyamin et al. (2009) rs4820268 A/G D521D serum iron haemoglobin transferrin saturation G = iron lowering Benyamin et al. (2009)

  30. Maternal genotype & Hb levels *p-value adjusted by gestational age at the time of measurement

  31. Maternal genotypes and iron supplementation P = 0.001 P = 4x10-5

  32. Maternal genetic score and child’s IQ

  33. Table 5. Maternal genotypes at SNPs in iron-related genes and full scale IQ of their children at 8 years of age.

  34. Limitations • instrument is associated with confounder • supplementation blurs association of genotype and Hb • small numbers for stratified analysis • HFE variants are rare

  35. Conclusions • HFE and TMPRSS6 variants were strongly associated with Hb levels. Not so TF rs3811647. • Genotypes associated with low Hb levels were also associated with higher risk of iron supplementation. • Mothers with rare HFE homozygote genotypes were more likely to have an educational level greater than O-level. • The association between iron-related genotypes and child’s IQ was strongest for women who took iron supplements during pregnancy. Adjustment by child’s genotype, maternal education, confounders and ancestry informative markers did not change this result. • Results suggest that exposure to low levels of iron in fetal life adversely affects brain development and therefore IQ in childhood. • Replication is needed.

  36. Lipids and cognition • lipids are vital for membrane biogenesis during cellular growth processes • On the other hand, elevated serum cholesterol is a well-known risk factor for cognitive decline, dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. • And higher serum LDL has been recently associated with decreased white matter integrity among healthy older adults.

  37. Genetic scores for Lipid levels • TG score = 27 SNPs • LDL-C score = 35 SNPs • HDL-C score = 44 SNPs • TC score = 46 SNPs • Weighted by effect sizes reported by Teslovich et al. (2010), Aulchenko et al. (2009) and Kettunen et al. (2012) • Additive model • Risk alleles increase TG, LDL-C and TC, and decrease HDL-C • Scores are not associated with confounders

  38. Association between offspring lipid genetic scores and plasma lipids, adjusted for age and sex.

  39. Association between offspring and maternal genotype scores and offspring IQ

  40. Quartiles of LDL-C score, LDL-C plasma levels and IQ in ALSPAC children.

  41. Conclusions • Scores have been developed which are strongly related to lipid levels. • Maternal lipid scores are not associated with offspring IQ • Offspring LDL score is associated with offspring IQ such that an increase in score increases IQ

  42. Acknowledgements Bristol Luisa ZuccoloOxford Carolina Bonilla David Smith Jean Golding Helga Refsum Andy Ness Yoav Ben-ShlomoAustralia David Gunnell Marie-Jo Brion George Davey Smith Craig Pennell Debbie LawlorRaine team and participants Amy Taylor Pauline Emmett Nic Timpson Beate St. Pourcain Kate Northstone Phil Lobb & Sue Ring ALSPAC team and participants

More Related