1 / 23

Why Grade Recommendations?

Why Grade Recommendations?. strong recommendations strong methods large precise effect few down sides of therapy weak recommendations weak methods imprecise estimate small effect substantial down sides. Why Grade Recommendations?. focus on the evidence

Download Presentation

Why Grade Recommendations?

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Why Grade Recommendations? • strong recommendations • strong methods • large precise effect • few down sides of therapy • weak recommendations • weak methods • imprecise estimate • small effect • substantial down sides

  2. Why Grade Recommendations? • focus on the evidence • alternate practitioner behavior • apply uniformly or • examine evidence themselves • consider patient circumstances • explore with the patient

  3. What Are We Grading? • methodological quality of evidence • likelihood of bias • strength of recommendations • must do to might do

  4. Grade of Evidence for Specific Question • too vague: alendronate in osteoporosis • patients – post-menopausal women • intervention • daily alendronate, dose 10 to 20 mg. • outcome – non-vertebral fractures

  5. What Influences Grade? • study design • basic • detailed design and execution • consistency • directness • reporting bias

  6. Methodological Quality • study design • randomization • quasi-randomization • observational study • detailed design and execution • concealment • balance in known prognostic factors • intention to treat principle observed • blinding • completeness of follow-up

  7. Methodologic Quality • consistency of results • if inconsistency, look for explanation • patients, intervention, outcome, methods • no clear threshold • size of effect, confidence intervals, statistical significance

  8. Relative Risk of Conversion to Sinus Rhythm Amiodarone vs Placebo or Digoxin or CCB Favours Control Favours Amiodarone ' Cowan 1986 1.11 (0.78 to 1.58) n = 34 ' Noc 1990 18.0 (1.17 to 276) n = 24 ' Capucci 1992 0.77 (0.37 to 1.62) n = 40 ' Cochrane 1994 1.15 (0.91 to 1.44) n = 30 ' Donovan 1995 1.05 (0.69 to 1.60) n = 64 ' Hou 1995 1.29 (0.97 to 1.72) n = 39 ' Kondili 1995 1.33 (0.71 to 2.47) n = 42 ' Galve 1996 1.13 (0.84 to 1.52) n = 100 ' Kontoyannis 1998 1.42 (1.08 to 1.85) n = 42 ' Bellandi 1999 1.41 (1.15 to 1.72) n = 120 ' Cotter 1999 1.43 (1.15 to 1.80) n = 100 ' Kochiadakis 1999 1.46 (1.19 to 1.78) n = 204 ' Bianconi 2000 2.04 (0.19 to 22.00) n = 83 ' Galperin 2000 33.70 (2.08 to 546) n = 95 ' Hohnloser 2000 3.13 (1.50 to 6.70) n = 203 ' Joseph 2000 1.32 (0.95 to 1.80) n = 75 ' Natale 2000 5.12 (2.60 to 10.00) n = 85 ' Peukurinen 2000 2.45 (1.49 to 4.02) n = 62 ' Vardas 2000 2.01 (1.55 to 2.60) n = 208 ' Villani 2000 4.75 (1.60 to 14.00) n = 120 ' Cybulski 2001 1.87 (1.37 to 2.55) n = 160 0.1 1 10 100

  9. Relative Risk of Conversion to Sinus Rhythm Amiodarone vs Placebo or Digoxin or CCB Favours Control Favours Amiodarone AF Duration > 48 hrs ' Bianconi 2000 2.04 (0.19 to 22.00) n = 83 ' Galperin 2000 33.70 (2.08 to 546) n = 95 ' Hohnloser 2000 3.13 (1.50 to 6.70) ' n = 203 Natale 2000 5.12 (2.60 to 10.00) n = 85 ' Villani 2000 4.75 (1.60 to 14.00) n = 120 ' Pooled Estimate 4.33 (2.76 to 6.77) AF Duration =/< 48 hrs ' Cowan 1986 1.11 (0.78 to 1.58) n = 34 ' Noc 1990 18.0 (1.17 to 276) n = 24 ' Capucci 1992 0.77 (0.37 to 1.62) n = 40 ' Cochrane 1994 1.15 (0.91 to 1.44) n = 30 ' Donovan 1995 1.05 (0.69 to 1.60) n = 64 ' Hou 1995 1.29 (0.97 to 1.72) n = 39 ' Kondili 1995 1.33 (0.71 to 2.47) n = 42 ' Galve 1996 1.13 (0.84 to 1.52) n = 100 ' Kontoyannis 1998 1.42 (1.08 to 1.85) n = 42 ' Bellandi 1999 1.41 (1.15 to 1.72) n = 120 ' Cotter 1999 1.43 (1.15 to 1.80) n = 100 ' Kochiadakis 1999 1.46 (1.19 to 1.78) n = 204 ' Joseph 2000 1.32 (0.95 to 1.80) n = 75 ' Peukurinen 2000 2.45 (1.49 to 4.02) n = 62 ' Vardas 2000 2.01 (1.55 to 2.60) ' n = 208 Cybulski 2001 1.87 (1.37 to 2.55) n = 160 ' Pooled Estimate 1.40 (1.25 to 1.57) 0.1 1 10 100

  10. Directness of Evidence • indirect treatment comparisons • interested in A versus B • have A versus C and B versus C • alendronate vs risedronate • both versus placebo, no head-to-head

  11. Four Levels of “Directness” • patients meet trials’ eligibility criteria • not included, but no reason to question • slight age difference, comorbidity, race • some question, bottom line applicable • valvular atrial fibrillation • serious question about biology • heart failure trials applicability to aortic stenosis

  12. Levels of Directness • interventions • same drugs and doses • similar drugs and doses • same class and biology • questionable class and biology • outcomes • same outcomes • similar (duration, quality of life) • less breathlessness for role function • laboratory exercise capacity for q of life

  13. Magnitude, Precision, Reporting Bias • magnitude not generally part of quality • but very large magnitude can upgrade • precision not generally part of quality • but sparse data can lower quality • reporting bias • high likelihood can lower quality

  14. Grading System • high quality well done RCT • intermediate quasi-RCT • low well done observational • insufficient anything else

  15. Moving Down • study execution • serious flaws can lower by one level • fatal flaws can lower by two levels • consistency • important inconsistency can lower by one level • directness of evidence • some uncertainty re relevance lower by one level • major uncertainty re relevance lower by two levels • selection bias • strong suspicion lower by 1 level

  16. Moving Up • very strong association, up 2 levels • insulin in diabetic ketoacidosis • strong, consistent association with no plausible confounders, up 2 levels • fluoride for preventing cavities • strong association can move up 1 level • ? HRT ?

  17. Risk/Benefit tradeoff • aspirin after myocardial infarction • meta-analysis of high quality RCTs • clear positive treatment effect • unequivocal recommendation to treat, uniform practice • accelerated TPA vs streptokinase after MI • very large single trial • clear positive effect • uncertainty whether to treat, varying practice

  18. Risk/Benefit tradeoff • Aspirin after myocardial infarction • 25% reduction in relative risk • side effects minimal, cost minimal • benefit obviously much greater than risk/cost • TPA vs streptokinase after MI • 12% reduction in relative risk • increased rate of intracranial hemorrhage • large increase in cost • benefit vs risk/cost a judgment call

  19. Strength of Recommendations Aspirin after MI – do it TPA rather than SK in MI -- probably do it -- probably don’t do it

  20. Grade of Recommendations • do it • probably do it • toss-up • probably don’t do it • don’t do it

  21. Judgment: Benefits vs Risks/Costs • seriousness of outcome • magnitude of effect • precision of treatment effect • risk of target event • risk of adverse events • cost of therapy • values

  22. Grades Translations • do it - 90% would choose it • possibly do it – 60% to 90% would choose • toss-up – 40% to 60% would choose • possibly don’t – 60% to 90% would decline • don’t - 90% would decline

  23. Conclusion • challenges in grading • balancing simplicity and complexity • judgement always required • consistent grading system required • must consider study design, execution, consistency, directness, reporting bias • magnitude, precision, only when extreme • balance of benefits and risks/cost • magnitude of effects; precision of effects; values and preferences

More Related