1 / 53

CAS LX 522 Syntax I

CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Week 4a. The DP and the geometry of trees. Previously, in LX522…. Sentences are made of constituents , themselves possibly made of constituents, etc. [The kid [who [dropped [the cookie]]]] [ate [it]]

yen-kelley
Download Presentation

CAS LX 522 Syntax I

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. CAS LX 522Syntax I Week 4a. The DP andthe geometry of trees

  2. Previously, in LX522… • Sentences are made of constituents, themselves possibly made of constituents, etc. • [The kid [who [dropped [the cookie]]]] [ate [it]] • Any account of syntactic knowledge will need to provide this kind of hierarchical structure. • Overall idea: The syntactic system we have in our head builds trees, such that all and only the trees that the system can build are grammatical. • To the extent that grammatical and ungrammatical sentences are distinguished in terms of the hierarchical structure, it must be part of the system.

  3. Minimalism • As we try to determine what the properties of this grammatical system are, we should assume as little as we can get away with. • Any language-like system that is going to create hierarchical structure is going to need something that takes two (or more, but let’s say that “two is simpler than any other number”)things and puts them together into something eligible for further combinations. • So, the machine that builds the trees has at least the operation Merge.

  4. X-theory • A phrase is a syntactic object formed by combining (merging) two syntactic objects, with the properties inherited from one of them (the headof the phrase). • A word is a syntactic object. maximalprojection intermediateprojection XP YP X specifier X ZP minimalprojection head complement

  5. X-theory • In the ’70s and ’80s, these ideas went by the name “X-theory”. • Every XP has exactly one: • head (a lexical item) • complement (another XP) • specifier (another XP) • for any X (N, V, A, P, I, etc.) maximalprojection intermediateprojection XP YP X specifier X ZP minimalprojection head complement

  6. Radford and the X(P) • To forestall confusion: lunch is both a minimal projection and a maximal projection. It functions as a phrase, an XP, but it has nothing in it but a head, an X. • Since you need to write something, Radford generally opts to write X for these X/XPs. VP V NP eat lunch

  7. Radford and the X(P) • In this class, and on my overheads, I will usually write X/XP as XP. You should do the same, but you should be aware that Radford does it differently. • In general, this will depend on whether the properties we are focusing on are those of phrases (XPs) or heads (Xs). In these ambiguous cases, it will almostinvariably turn out thatthey act like phraseswith respect to what weare focusing on. VP V NP eat lunch

  8. Radford and the X(P) • Another similar comment pertains to the status of IP below. It is an IP. It is not an I. It’s true that it will be an I after we combine Pat with the IP, but it isn’t yet. Cf. Radford p. 120. N IP Pat I VP will V NP eat lunch

  9. X, X, XP • In English, the head and the complement always seem to come in that order: head-complement. • at lunch (P NP = PP) • eat lunch (V NP = VP) • will eat lunch (I VP = IP) • But here, languages differ.English is a head-first (orhead-initial) language. PP P NP at lunch

  10. X, X, XP • In Japanese, the head follows the complement. Japanese is head-final. • ringo-o tabeta (NP V = VP)apple ate • toshokan de (NP P = PP)library at • This seems to be aparameter thatdistinguishes languages(the head parameter) PP NP P toshokan de

  11. X, X, XP • Whether the specifier comes before X or after is independent of whether the head comes before the complement. • Specifiers are overwhelmingly initial, although a few languages may be best analyzed as having final specifiers (sometimes). • E.g., Japanese, which ishead-final, neverthelesshas initial specifiers. IP NP I Ringo-ga VP I ringo-o tabe- ta

  12. Narrowing in: NP? • Traditionally, a phrase like the students is called a noun phrase and written as NP. • What does this imply about the structure? • What category is students? • What category is the? • Which one is the head? • Where is the other one?

  13. Narrowing in: NP? • Traditionally, a phrase like the students is called a noun phrase and written as NP. • What does this imply about the structure? • What category is students? • What category is the? • Which one is the head? • Where is the other one? • Is this Japanese? ? NP DP N the students

  14. Narrowing in: NP? • There are a couple of problems with this. • There’s the headedness problem • The syntactic object that combines with the head is the complement, not the specifier. • (Note: There is a way out of this, we’ll see it later) • Supposing that the is a whole DP is suspicious, because it can never bemodified by anything.Modifiability is a signatureproperty of phrases. ? NP DP N the students

  15. DP! • If the studentsis not an NP, it must be a DP. • It’s head-initial, like English should be. • The NP can of course be modified (happy students). • There are several reasons to think that the students is a DP and not an NP, even better than these two, which we’ll see indue course (…at the appropriatejuncture, in the fullness of time). ! DP D NP the students

  16. DP • Consider the genitive (possessive) ’sin English: • John’s hat • The student’s sandwich • The man from Australia’s book • The man on the hill by the tree’s binoculars • Notice that the ’s attaches to the whole possessor phrase—in the last two examples, it isn’t even attached to the head noun (it’s the man’s book and binoculars, not Australia’s or the tree’s, after all). • This is not a noun suffix. It seems more like a little word that signals possession, standing between the possessor and the possessee. (Recall, it’s a clitic).

  17. DP • It seems to be impossible to have both a ’s and a determiner. • *The building’s the roof • Cf. The roof of the building • *The tiger’s the eye • Determiners like the and the possession marker ’s seem to be in complementary distribution—if one appears, the other cannot. • You may recall a similar pattern from a couple of weeks ago. What was the explanation for the *s? • The big fluffy pink rabbit*The that rabbit • *The my rabbit *Every my rabbit

  18. DP • *Pat’s the big fluffy pink rabbit • This would make sense if both theand ’s are instances of the category D; DP can have only one head.

  19. Possessors • This suggests a structure like this for possession phrases: • The possessor DP is in the specifier of DP. And of course, this can be as complex a DP as we like, e.g., the very hungry linguistics student by the tree with the purple flowers over there. • The possessed NP is the complement of D. DP DP D D D NP NP the student ’s book

  20. Recursion • Another noteworthy aspect of the possessor phrase is its recursive property. • The possessor is a DP in the specifier of DP. That means that the DP possessor could have a possessor too… • The student’s father’s book • The student’s mother’s brother’s roommate

  21. X-bar theory: DP DP • The student’s mother’s brother’s roommate DP D DP D D NP DP D ’s roommate D NP D D NP NP ’s brother the student ’s mother

  22. X-bar theory: DP • What do we do with apparently simple “NPs” like John or students (e.g., Students in the class complained bitterly)? Are these NPs or DPs? • Well, there are two options… • One: They are NPs. • Pro: Just as they appear. • Con: Subjects, objects, etc. can be either NPs or DPs. • Two: They are DPs. • Pro: Subject, objects, etc. are always DPs. • Con: Not obvious from the surface pronunciation.

  23. X-bar theory: DP • What do we do with apparently simple “NPs” like John or students (e.g., Students in the class complained bitterly)? Are these NPs or DPs? • Well, there are two options… • One: They are NPs. • Pro: Just as they appear. • Con: Subjects, objects, etc. can be either NPs or DPs. • Two: They are DPs. • Pro: Subject, objects, etc. are always DPs. • Con: Not obvious from the surface pronunciation.

  24. Pronouns • Consider: me, you, him (or I, you, he)… • Since a pronoun can be the subject of a sentence (e.g., I left), a pronoun must be part of a DP. • For pronouns, however, there’s some reason to believe that they actually head the DP. That is, that the pronoun I is a D. • That is, not PRN as we’d called it up until now, but actually in the same category as the and ’s.

  25. X-bar theory: Pronouns • Consider the following: • You politicians are all alike. • We linguists need to stick together. • The media always mocks us academics. • These seem to have a pronoun followed by a noun inside the DP; we can make sense of this if the pronoun is a D which can optionally take an NP complement. DP D NP we linguists

  26. X-bar theory: Bare nouns and proper names • How about something like students (in Students poured out of the auditorium at noon) or John (in John went for a walk)? • For students, we want to believe that it is an instance of the N category (in order to make sense of the students or we students or John’s students. But if this N is contained in a DP (the complement of a D head), where is the D? • In order to maintain consistency, we’ll suppose that in bare nouns D is present but null(it has no phonological representation; we write this as Ø).

  27. X-bar theory: Bare nouns and proper names • So for the bare noun students, we have a structure like that shown here. • As for proper names like Pat, we will assume that they are essentially like students. • Why? Why not like I, me, them? DP D NP Ø students DP D NP Ø John

  28. Trees • We will be working with trees a lot, and the geometry of trees will be quite important. We need some terminology to talk about the parts of trees.

  29. Trees An abstract tree structure… A B C E D

  30. Trees • The “joints” of the tree are nodes. The nodes here are labeled (with node labels). A B C E D

  31. Trees • The “joints” of the tree are nodes. The nodes here are labeled (with node labels). • Nodes are connected by branches. A B C E D

  32. Trees • The “joints” of the tree are nodes. The nodes here are labeled (with node labels). • Nodes are connected by branches. • The node at the top of the tree (with no branches above it) is called the root node. A is the root node. A B C E D

  33. Trees • Nodes with no branches beneath them are called terminal nodes. • B, D, E are terminal nodes. A B C E D

  34. Trees • Nodes with no branches beneath them are called terminal nodes. • B, D, E are terminal nodes. • Nodes with branches beneath them are called nonterminal nodes. • A, C are nonterminal nodes. A B C E D

  35. Tree relations • A node X dominates nodes below it on the tree; these are the nodes which would be pulled along if you grabbed the node X and pulled it off of the page. A B C E D

  36. Tree relations • A node X dominates nodes below it on the tree; these are the nodes which would be pulled along if you grabbed the node X and pulled it off of the page. • C dominates D and E. A B C C E D E D

  37. Tree relations • Remind you of anything?To briefly reconnect with actual language data, it seems as if you do something to C (like pull it off the page), it affects D and E as a unit. A B C C E D E D

  38. Tree relations • A set of terminal nodes is a constituent if they are all dominated by the same node and no other terminal nodes are dominated by that node. • “D E” is a constituent. • “B D” is not. A B C C E D E D

  39. Tree relations • A node X immediately dominates a node Y if X dominates Y and is connected by only one branch. • A immediately dominatesB and C. A B C E D

  40. Tree relations • A node X immediately dominates a node Y if X dominates Y and is connected by only one branch. • A immediately dominatesB and C. • A is also sometimes called the mother of B and C. A B C E D

  41. Tree relations • A node which shares the same mother as a node X is sometimes called the sister of X. • B is the sister of C. • C is the sister of B. • D is the sister of E. A B C E D

  42. Tree relations • A node X c-commands its sisters and the nodes dominated by its sisters. A B C E D

  43. Tree relations • A node X c-commands its sisters and the nodes dominated by its sisters. • B c-commands C, D, and E. A B C E D

  44. Tree relations • A node X c-commands its sisters and the nodes dominated by its sisters. • B c-commands C, D, and E. • D c-commands E. A B C E D

  45. Tree relations • A node X c-commands its sisters and the nodes dominated by its sisters. • B c-commands C, D, and E. • D c-commands E. • C c-commands B. A B C E D

  46. X-bar configurations • The complement is the sister of the head. • The specifier is the sister of X that is a daughter of XP. XP YP X ZP X

  47. Precedence • The tree also encodes the linear order of the terminal nodes.

  48. Precedence • The tree also encodes the linear order of the terminal nodes. • The is pronounced before students. NP D N the students

  49. Precedence • The tree also encodes the linear order of the terminal nodes. • The is pronounced before students. • Saw is pronounced before the and students. VP V NP saw D N the students

  50. Precedence • That is, V is pronounced before NP, meaning V is pronounced before all of the terminal nodes dominated by NP. VP V NP saw D N the students

More Related