1 / 59

Manifestation of Body Reference in the Sense of Verticality

Manifestation of Body Reference in the Sense of Verticality. Ronald Kaptein. October 6, 2003. Introduction. Classical experimental results Mittelstaedt’s model Unresolved issues Hysteresis Bistability Objectives of present study. Introduction. Classical studies.

terris
Download Presentation

Manifestation of Body Reference in the Sense of Verticality

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Manifestation of Body Reference in the Sense of Verticality Ronald Kaptein October 6, 2003

  2. Introduction • Classical experimental results • Mittelstaedt’s model • Unresolved issues • Hysteresis • Bistability • Objectives of present study

  3. Introduction Classical studies

  4. Paradox in classical studies When tilted in the dark • Subjects make no systematic errors in estimating their body orientation • Subjects make systematic errors in estimating the direction of vertical

  5. Tilt dependent pattern of errors (rear view)

  6. Introduction Mittelstaedt’s model

  7. Assumptions • The gravity signal is derived from the otoliths

  8. Assumptions • Errors would occur if no corrections are made for unequal sizes of the otolith organs S is the ratio of the gains of the saccule and the utricle

  9. Idiotropic vector • A constant head-fixed bias signal (idiotropic vector) solves this problem for small tilts • But it increases the error for large tilts

  10. Mittelstaedt model • This head-fixed bias can be seen as a strategy to decrease errors in the daily encountered tilt range

  11. Introduction Unresolved issues

  12. Hysteresis • Visual vertical settings for CW and CCW rotations to same tilt angle are different Indicates involvement of dynamic factors, which conflicts with Mittelstaedt model Udo de Haes & Schöne (1970)

  13. Bistability • Anecdotal reports of bistable visual-vertical settings at large tilts. Anecdotally reported setting Classical & predicted setting Fischer (1930) Udo de Haes and Schöne (1970)

  14. Objectives of present study • Quantative verification of hysteresis and bistability. • Check possible connection between hysteresis and bistability. • Check if hysteresis and bistability are also present in body-tilt estimations

  15. Method

  16. Vestibular roll rotation • Subjects are rotated to an angle between 0 and 360º, clockwise (CW) or counterclockwise (CCW). Testing begins 30 s after stop.

  17. Paradigms • Visual vertical paradigm • Subjects have to indicate the vertical by adjusting a polarized luminous line (6 subjects, 3 naive) • Body tilt paradigm • Subjects have to verbally indicate their perceived body orientation using a clock scale (4 subjects, 1 naive)

  18. Results • Visual vertical • Body tilt • Summary main findings

  19. Results Visual-vertical settings

  20. Results visual-vertical settings • Deviation from Mittelstaedt prediction and classical data at large tilts.

  21. Expected visual-vertical settings • Expected results according to Mittelstaedt model:

  22. Results of typical subject • Bistable settings and major departure from Mittelstaedt prediction at large tilts (gray zone).  CW

  23. Results of typical subject • Hysteresis negligible  CW o CCW

  24. Results of all subjects • 5 of the 6 subjects show bistability - CW - CCW

  25. Mean results of visual-vertical settings • Hysteresis also negligible in overal mean - CW - CCW

  26. Pictorial illustration of bistability

  27. Results Body-tilt estimates

  28. Results body-tilt estimates of typical subject • No bistability at large tilts  CW

  29. Results body-tilt estimates of typical subject • Weak signs of hysteresis  CW o CCW

  30. Body-tilt estimates of all subjects • None of the subjects shows bistability - CW - CCW

  31. Mean body-tilt estimate • Overall means show clear hysteresis: - CW - CCW

  32. Main results • Bistable response patterns are robust in the visual-vertical task, but absent in the body-tilt task • Weak hysteresis in body-tilt estimates, none in visual-vertical results.

  33. Discussion • Comparison of visual vertical and body tilt • Hysteresis • Modelling bistability

  34. Discussion Comparison of visual vertical and body tilt

  35. Comparison of performance in the two tasks • No correlation between subjective visual vertical and subjective body tilt CCW CW SVV SBT --------

  36. Errors in visual vertical do not result from wrong tilt estimates • Correlation not significant (R=-0.03) CW CCW

  37. Discussion Hysteresis

  38. No hysteresis in visual vertical • Hysteresis in body-tilt but not in visual-vertical results - CW - CCW

  39. Hysteresis • Hysteresis in body-tilt percept • May indicate that estimated body-tilt is partly based on path integration of canals, which will adapt during constant velocity rotation. • No hysteresis in visual-vertical settings • The results of Udo de Haes & Schone are not confirmed. Mittelstaedt’s assumption that the final tilt angle is the important variable is supported.

  40. Discussion Modelling bistability

  41. Bistability • The bistable transition near 135º is a robust finding in nearly all subjects. • The anecdotal reports of bistability (Fischer (1930), Udo de Haes &Schöne (1970)) are confirmed and quantified.

  42. Manifestation of body reference • All data can be described by the influence of a body reference, which is head- or feet-directed.

  43. Mittelstaedt model cannot account for all data • Fitting Mittelstaedt on all data clearly fails: M=0.2±0.2 S=0.97±0.05 R²=0.26

  44. Mittelstaedt can account for small and medium tilt data • Fitting Mittelstaedt on white zone does not account for the gray zone: M=0.32±0.02 S=0.61±0.04 R²=0.70

  45. Descriptive model • Allowing the idiotropic to be different in the two tilt zones works: M1= 0.33±0.02 M2= -1.5±0.4 switch = 133±1 S= 0.60±0.03 R²= 0.68

  46. Descriptive model • Different idiotropics for the two tilt regions can fit the data: Head-directed idiotropic: Feet-directed Idiotropic:

  47. Possible mechanisms underlying bistability • Why? Reports from subjects about the nature of the task gives an indication: • For small and medium tilts the task is easy and more or less automatic. • For large tilts the task is difficult and subjects try to use every cue availabe, making the task more cognitive. • The brain may use different strategies (systems) in the two tilt zones.

  48. Possible mechanisms underlying bistability • Default brainstem mechanism • Operates on assumption that tilt is in normal working range (head-directed idiotropic, Mittelstaedt model) • Cognitive system • Takes over when tilt is beyond normal working range.

  49. Cognitive system uses perceived body-tilt signal CCW CW SVV SBT --------

  50. What determines the transition angle? • Transition near =90º 2 1

More Related