1 / 13

D.M. Dwyer

Flavour preference learning and palatability: Impairments by basolateral, but not central, amygdala lesions. D.M. Dwyer. Flavour preference learning. A preference for a neutral or disliked flavour can be created in a several ways. For example: Pairing with nutrients

melody
Download Presentation

D.M. Dwyer

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Flavour preference learning and palatability: Impairments by basolateral, but not central, amygdala lesions. D.M. Dwyer

  2. Flavour preference learning • A preference for a neutral or disliked flavour can be created in a several ways. For example: • Pairing with nutrients • Pairing with a particularly palatable flavour. • Conditioning produces a preference for the CS+ over the CS- and to some extent an increase in CS+ consumption in extinction. • But beyond the consumption differences little is known about the effects of preference conditioning or the mechanisms underpinning this consumption change.

  3. Change in CS palatability? • The examination of orofacial responses in the taste-reactivity test is one example of analyses that go beyond mere consumption to examine “palatability”. • Some, but not all, conditioned preferences are accompanied by an increase in CS palatability. • Compare Myers & Sclafani (2001) with Myers & Sclafani (2003). • But taste reactivity method is less than ideal for many reasons. • E.g. it is very labour intensive, creates a context change, only examines short exposures to fluids.

  4. Microstructural analysis of licking • Rats consuming fluids produce sustained runs of rapidly occurring rhythmic licks (clusters) separated by pauses of varying lengths. • Mean cluster size has a positive monotonic relationship with concentration of sucrose (& other palatable flavours) and a negative relationship with concentration of unpalatable quinine. • This supports the idea that cluster size is an index of stimulus palatability (and there is further support from pharmacological studies). • Other factors (e.g. inter-lick interval within clusters) relatively unaffected by most manipulations.

  5. Current aims • Lick analysis has a number of practical and theoretical advantages over taste reactivity. • E.g. it can be automated & applied to entire drinking sessions without any impact on the animal observed. • Current study aimed at examining lesion effects in flavour preference conditioning (basolateral v central amygdala) with fructose as the reinforcer. • Almost no cross-talk between microstructure & taste reactivity literatures. • So current study should also re-emphasise common ground between these methods of assessing palatability.

  6. Experimental background • Lesions of the whole amygdala have been shown to completely prevent flavour preference learning with nutrient and palatability reinforcers • IG maltodextrin: Touzani & Sclafani, 2005; Oral sucrose: Gilbert et al., 2003; Oral saccharin: Sakai & Yamamoto, 2001. • But large lesions mean rather unspecific neural basis for these effects. • Consider the debate about whether damage to fibres of passage underpins the inconsistent effects of amygdala lesions on CTA. • Thus current study examined effects of BLA and CN lesions on palatability based preferences.

  7. Methods • Naïve hooded Lister: 12 sham lesions, 15 quinolinic acid lesions of the BLA, 14 NMDA lesions of the CN. • Note: originally 20 per lesion group but N reduced after histology. • All 85% of free feeding weight during training and test. • CSs = 0.05% grape & cherry Kool Aid plus 0.1% saccharin. US = 8% Fructose. • 4 CS+ and 4 CS- sessions given. All sessions 30min. • 2-bottle test (CS+ v CS-) then 2 × 1-bottle tests (one each with CS+ & CS-) in extinction. Pattern of tests repeated and data averaged over test sessions.

  8. 2-Bottle Consumption 12 10 8 CS+ Consumption (g) 6 CS- 4 2 0 Sham BLA CN Lesion • Preferential consumption of CS+ over CS- in all groups. • But size of preference reduced in BLA group compared to both Sham and CN (which did not differ).

  9. 1-Bottle Consumption 14 12 10 CS+ 8 Consumption (g) 6 CS- 4 2 0 Sham BLA CN Lesion • Clear interaction between lesion condition & CS. • Sham and CN lesions show higher consumption of CS+ than CS- but BLA does not.

  10. 1-Bottle Cluster Size 40 35 30 25 Cluster Size 20 15 10 5 0 Sham BLA CN Lesion • Clear interaction between lesion condition & CS. • Sham and CN lesions show higher cluster sizes with CS+ than CS- but BLA does not.

  11. Results Summary • Analysis of cluster size suggests that preference conditioning increaces palatability of the CS+. • BLA lesions reduced, but did not totally prevent, palatability based preference learning. • Sham lesioned animals displayed enhanced palatability reaction to CS+. This not seen in BLA lesioned animals but is seen in CN lesions.

  12. Discussion • Conditioned change in cluster size consistent with examination of orofacial reactions. • Supports the idea that both techniques are sensitive to the same sort of manipulations. • BLA lesion prevents conditioned changes in palatability and additional consumption of the CS+ in 1-bottle tests. • Thus palatability change appears to contribute to additional CS+ consumption in 1-bottle tests. • Palatability change cannot underpin all of the conditioned change in CS preference produced by pairing the CS+ with a palatable taste as BLA animals still showed some preference conditioning without palatability change.

  13. General Summary • Many other applications of the lick-analysis technique are possible. For example I have used this to examine: • The effects of NMDA antagonists on palatability. • How taste aversions based on different mechanisms (e.g. nausea, activity or drugs of abuse) can actually have different effects of the palatability of the avoided food. • How contrasts between foods can lead to an inflation of the palatability differences between them. • Whether chronic social stress might reduce the hedonic reaction to sucrose. • Basically, in any rat (possibly also mouse) model where the question of “how nice” a substance might be is raised, lick analysis might provide an answer.

More Related