1 / 35

User Testing: Choosing Participants, Designing the Test, and Analyzing Data

This lecture provides an in-depth look at user testing, including how to select participants, design the test, and analyze the collected data. It also explores the importance of quantitative methods in evaluating user interfaces.

lucretiar
Download Presentation

User Testing: Choosing Participants, Designing the Test, and Analyzing Data

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. CS 160: Lecture 16 Professor John Canny Spring 2004

  2. Outline • More on user testing • Choosing participants • Designing the test • Basics of quantitative methods • Collecting data • Analyzing the data

  3. Jakob Nielsen Why do User Testing? • Can’t tell how good or bad UI is until? • people use it! • Other methods are based on evaluators who: • may know too much • may not know enough (about tasks, etc.) • Summary Hard to predict what real users will do

  4. Choosing Participants • Representative of eventual users in terms of • job-specific vocabulary / knowledge • tasks • If you can’t get real users, get approximation • system intended for doctors • get medical students • system intended for electrical engineers • get engineering students • Use incentives to get participants

  5. Incentives • Paying every subject a fixed amount can be too expensive. • A small chance at a large reward is a good incentive – e.g. subjects get a 1/n chance to receive an MP3 player. • Offer to tell them the results ofthe study. • Free software for software companies. • Some courses require participation asresearch subjects.

  6. Ethical Considerations • Sometimes tests can be distressing • users have left in tears (embarrassed by mistakes) • You have a responsibility to alleviate • make voluntary with informed consent • avoid pressure to participate • will not affect their job status either way • let them know they can stop at any time • stress that you are testing the system, not them • make collected data as anonymous as possible

  7. Ethical Considerations • Get human subjects approval if needed – typically if results are going to be published. • Universities always have humansubjects panels, so do governmentagencies.

  8. User Test Proposal • A report that contains • objective • description of system being testing • task environment & materials • participants • methodology • tasks • test measures • Get approved & then reuse for final report

  9. Qualitative vs. Quantitative Studies • Qualitative: What we’ve been doing so far: • Contextual Inquiry: trying to understand user’s tasks and their conceptual model. • Usability Studies: looking for critical incidents in a user interface. • In general, we use qualitative methods to: • Understand what’s going on, look for problems, or get a rough idea of the usability of an interface.

  10. Qualitative vs. Quantitative Studies • Quantitative: • Use to reliably measure something. • Requires us to know how to measure it. • Examples: • Time to complete a task. • Average number of errors on a task. • Users’ ratings of an interface *: • Ease of use, elegance, performance, robustness, speed,… * - You could argue that users’ perception of speed, error rates etc is more important than their actual values.

  11. Quantitative Methods • Very often, we want to compare values for two different designs (which is faster?). This requires some statistical methods. • We begin by defining some quantities to measure - variables.

  12. Variable types • Independent Variables: the ones you control • Aspects of the interface design • Characteristics of the testers • Discrete: A, B or C • Continuous: Time between clicks for double-click • Dependent variables: the ones you measure • Time to complete tasks • Number of errors

  13. Deciding on Data to Collect • Two types of data • process data • observations of what users are doing & thinking • bottom-line data • summary of what happened (time, errors, success…) • i.e., the dependent variables

  14. Process Data vs. Bottom Line Data • Focus on process data first • gives good overview of where problems are • Bottom-line doesn’t tell you where to fix • just says: “too slow”, “too many errors”, etc. • Hard to get reliable bottom-line results • need many users for statistical significance

  15. The “Thinking Aloud” Method • Need to know what users are thinking, not just what they are doing • Ask users to talk while performing tasks • tell us what they are thinking • tell us what they are trying to do • tell us questions that arise as they work • tell us things they read • Make a recording or take good notes • make sure you can tell what they were doing

  16. Thinking Aloud (cont.) • Prompt the user to keep talking • “tell me what you are thinking” • Only help on things you have pre-decided • keep track of anything you do give help on • Recording • use a digital watch/clock • take notes, plus if possible • record audio and video (or even event logs)

  17. Break

  18. Some statistics • Variables X & Y • A relation (hypothesis) e.g. X > Y • We would often like to know if a relation is true • e.g. X = time taken by novice users • Y = time taken by users with some training • To find out if the relation is true we do experiments to get lots of x’s and y’s (observations) • Suppose avg(x) > avg(y), or that most of the x’s are larger than all of the y’s. What does that prove?

  19. Significance • The significance or p-value of an outcome is the probability that it happens by chance if the relation does not hold. • E.g. p = 0.05 means that there is a 1/20 chance that the observation happens if the hypothesis is false. • So the smaller the p-value, the greater the significance.

  20. Significance • For instance p = 0.001 means there is a 1/1000 chance that the observation would happen if the hypothesis is false. So the hypothesis is almost surely true. • Significance increases with number of trials. • CAVEAT: You have to make assumptions about the probability distributions to get good p-values. There is always an implied model of user performance.

  21. Normal distributions • Many variables have a Normal distribution • At left is the density, right is the cumulative prob. • Normal distributions are completely characterized by their mean and variance (mean squared deviation from the mean).

  22. Normal distributions • The difference between two independent normal variables is also a normal variable, whose variance is the sum of the variances of the distributions. • Asserting that X > Y is the same as (X-Y) > 0, whose probability we can read off from the curve.

  23. Analyzing the Numbers • Example: prove that task 1 is faster on design A than design B. • Suppose the average time for design B is 20% higher than A. • Suppose subjects’ times in the study have a std. dev. which is 30% of their mean time (typical). • How many subjects are needed?

  24. Analyzing the Numbers • Example: prove that task 1 is faster on design A than design B. • Suppose the average time for design B is 20% higher than A. • Suppose subjects’ times in the study have a std. dev. which is 30% of their mean time (typical). • How many subjects are needed? • Need at least 13 subjects for significance p=0.01 • Need at least 22 subjects for significance p=0.001 • (assumes subjects use both designs)

  25. Analyzing the Numbers (cont.) • i.e. even with strong (20%) difference, need lots of subjects to prove it. • Usability test data is quite variable • 4 times as many tests will only narrow range by 2x • breadth of range depends on sqrt of # of test users • This is when online methods become useful • easy to test w/ large numbers of users (e.g., Landay’s NetRaker system)

  26. Lies, damn lies and statistics… • A common mistake (made by famous HCI researchers *) • Increasing n (number of trials) by running each subject several times. • No! the analysis only works when trials are independent. • All the trials for one subject are dependent, because that subject may be faster/slower/less error-prone than others. * - making this error will not help you become a famous HCI researcher .

  27. Statistics with care: • What you can do to get better significance: • Run each subject several times, compute the average for each subject. • Run the analysis as usual on subjects’ average times, with n = number of subjects. • This decreases the per-subject variance, while keeping data independent.

  28. Measuring User Preference • How much users like or dislike the system • can ask them to rate on a scale of 1 to 10 • or have them choose among statements • “best UI I’ve ever…”, “better than average”… • hard to be sure what data will mean • novelty of UI, feelings, not realistic setting, etc. • If many give you low ratings -> trouble • Can get some useful data by asking • what they liked, disliked, where they had trouble, best part, worst part, etc. (redundant questions)

  29. B A Using Subjects • Between subjects experiment • Two groups of test users • Each group uses only 1 of the systems • Within subjects experiment • One group of test users • Each person uses both systems

  30. Between subjects • Two groups of testers, each use 1 system • Advantages: • Users only have to use one system (practical). • No learning effects. • Disadvantages: • Per-user performance differences confounded with system differences: • Much harder to get significant results (many more subjects needed). • Harder to even predict how many subjects will be needed (depends on subjects).

  31. Within subjects • One group of testers who use both systems • Advantages: • Much more significance for a given number of test subjects. • Disadvantages: • Users have to use both systems (two sessions). • Order and learning effects (can be minimized by experiment design).

  32. Example • Same experiment as before: • System B is 20% slower than A • Subjects have 30% std. dev. in their times. • Within subjects: • Need 13 subjects for significance p = 0.01 • Between subjects: • Typically require 52 subjects for significance p = 0.01. • But depending on the subjects, we may get lower or higher significance.

  33. Experimental Details • Order of tasks • choose one simple order (simple -> complex) • unless doing within groups experiment • Training • depends on how real system will be used • What if someone doesn’t finish • assign very large time & large # of errors • Pilot study • helps you fix problems with the study • do 2, first with colleagues, then with real users

  34. Reporting the Results • Report what you did & what happened • Images & graphs help people get it!

  35. Summary • Use real tasks & representative participants • Be ethical & treat your participants well • Want to know what people are doing & why • i.e., collect process data • Using bottom line data requires more users to get statistically reliable results

More Related