1 / 1

Relationship of explanatory flexibility and explanatory style to each other and to depression, anxiety, and emotion regu

Relationship of explanatory flexibility and explanatory style to each other and to depression, anxiety, and emotion regulation Daniel Button, Michael T. Moore, Shane A. Mares, & David M. Fresco. ABSTRACT

libitha
Download Presentation

Relationship of explanatory flexibility and explanatory style to each other and to depression, anxiety, and emotion regu

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Relationship of explanatory flexibility and explanatory style to each other and to depression, anxiety, and emotion regulation Daniel Button, Michael T. Moore, Shane A. Mares, & David M. Fresco • ABSTRACT • The present study sought to explore the relationship of explanatory flexibility to explanatory style and to examine the differences in explanatory style as a function of explanatory flexibility, with regard to depression, anxiety, emotion regulation and emotional intelligence. Two samples of undergraduate students were collected, sample 1 (n = 725) completed the Attributional Style Questionnaire (ASQ; Peterson et al., 1982), which serves as both a measure of explanatory style and explanatory flexibility. Sample 2 (n = 202) completed measures of both explanatory style, explanatory flexibility, depression, anxiety, emotion regulation, and emotional intelligence. In sample 1, explanatory style and explanatory flexibility were found to be independent of one another, with this nearly result (p = .06) corresponding to Cohen’s convention for a small effect size (Cohen, 1992; Cohen’s w = .10). Sample 2 also revealed that explanatory flexibility and explanatory style were largely independent, although the nonsignificant result in sample 2 (p = .14) suggests this to a lesser extent (w = .25). The interaction of explanatory flexibility and explanatory style was also assessed in sample 2, and it was found that: (1) rigid versus flexible pessimists were found to exhibit higher levels of anxious arousal (d = -.31), (2) rigid optimists showed a trend towards higher levels of fear of potent positive emotions (d = .27), as well as significantly higher levels of fear of anxiety and a greater need to control their emotions, (d = .32), and (3) flexible optimists were found to seek novel situations more (d = .30), have a higher degree of engagement with their surroundings (d = -.39), exhibit more open-mindedness (d = -.32), and show a trend towards greater willingness to experience and express positive emotions (d = -.28) than rigid optimists. All findings corresponded to between small and medium effect sizes (d = .25 – .39). • DISCUSSION • These findings indicate that explanatory flexibility is a distinct construct from explanatory style, with differential relationships with extra-test measures of negative affect, emotional intelligence, and emotion regulation. Specifically: • Rigid pessimists evidenced higher levels of anxiety • Rigid optimists evidenced higher levels of fear of both positive and negative emotions, and increased need to control these emotions as a result • Flexible optimists evidenced increased engagement with their environment, open-mindedness,seeking of novel situations, and the experience and expression of positive emotions • Limitations • Participants consisted of relatively high- functioning college students, resulting in uncertain generalizability to the general public • Future Studies • Replicating the current study utilizing a more representative population • INTRODUCTION • The reformulated learned helplessness theory (Abramson et al., 1978) as well as the hopelessness theory (Abramson, et al., 1989) are cognitive diathesis-stress models of depression which follow from the original learned helplessness theory (Seligman, 1974). The reformulated learned helplessness theory (Abramson, et al., 1978) infused attribution theory into the original model and posited that the causal attributions that individuals assign to events in their lives would confer protection from- or risk for depression. • Specifically, the attributional component of the reformulated learned helplessness theory, referred to as explanatory style, is defined as an individual’s relatively stable tendencies in assigning causes to negative events. Individuals who attribute negative life events to internal, (i.e. “It’s all my fault”), stable (i.e. “It will be around for a long time”) and global (i.e. “It will affect everything I do”) causes are thought to be more vulnerable to depression when confronted by these events in the reformulated learned helplessness theory. Hopelessness theory retains the causal attribution component but de-emphasizes the internality dimension in favor of the stability and globality dimensions.Considerable evidence supports the relationship of cognitive content to the etiology, maintenance, and treatment of emotional disorders (Alloy, et al., 1999; Abela & Seligman, 2000). However, this focus on cognitive content has left room for inquiries into the nature of cognitive processes in predicting depression. • Explanatory flexibility (Fresco et al., 2000) has been defined as the process by which attributions to life events are made. Explanatory flexibility assesses the relative variability of respondents’ self-ratings on the stable and global dimensions of the Attributional Style Questionnaire. Specifically, individuals with a large standard deviation are able to attribute events to a variety of causes, and are regarded as flexible, whereas individuals with a small standard deviation are regarded as rigid. • The current study sought to extend previous research showing that explanatory flexibility is an independent construct from explanatory style (Fresco & Craighead, 2002; Fresco & Seligman, 2003; Fresco et al., 2000) and exploring the differences in explanatory style as a function of explanatory flexibility, with regard to depression, anxiety, emotion regulation and emotional intelligence. • RESULTS • To examine the independence of explanatory style and explanatory flexibility, both were divided into tertiles, thereby creating a 3x3 matrix. A Chi-Square analysis was then conducted, revealing a generally comparable distribution across the 3x3 matrix. The results were: • In sample 1: 2(4, N = 725) = 9.27, p = .06, w = .10 • In sample 2: 2(4, N = 202) = 6.98, p = .14, w = .25 • Participants were then classified into one of 9 groups based on their tertile membership status ranging from rigid optimists to flexible pessimists. A series of one-way ANOVAs were conducted to determine any relationships between levels of optimism and flexibility to other criterion measures. Two non-orthogonal planned contrasts were conducted to permit comparisons of rigid optimists to flexible optimists and between rigid pessimists to flexible pessimists. Omnibus and the contrast analysis revealed no group differences in depression as a function of optimism and flexibility status. • In contrast 1, rigid versus flexible pessimists were found to exhibit higher levels of anxious arousal, [t(193) = -2.13, p = .04, d = -.31]. No other significant differences were detected between flexibility and depression for either optimists or pessimists • In contrast 2, rigid optimists showed a trend towards higher levels of fear of potent positive emotions [t(193) = 1.85, p = .07, d = .27], as well as significantly higher levels of fear of anxiety and a greater need to control their emotions, [t(193) = 2.24, p = .03, d = .32]. Flexible optimists were found to seek novel situations more [t(192) = -2.08, p = .04, d = -.30], have a higher degree of engagement with their surroundings [t(192) = -2.70, p < .01, d = -.39], exhibit more open-mindedness [t(192) = -2.23, p = .03, d = -.32], and show a trend towards greater willingness to experience and express positive emotions [t(192) = -1.91, p = .06, d = -.28] than rigid optimists. • All findings corresponded to between small and medium effect sizes (Cohen’s d = .25 to .39) • METHODS • Participants • Sample 1 • 725 undergraduate students • 33% male, 67% female • 1% Asian, 13% African American, 82% Caucasian, 2% Hispanic, 2% Other • Participants ranged from 16-78 years of age (M = 19.37 SD = 4.52) • Sample 2 • 202 undergraduate students • 38% male, 62% female • Participants ranged from 17-54 years of age (M = 22.04, SD = 8.02) • Measures • Attributional Style Questionnaire (ASQ; Peterson et al., 1982) • Mood & Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (Watson & Clark, 1991) • Affective Control Scale (Williams, Chambless, & Ahrens, 1997) • Toronto Alexithymia Scale (Bagby et al., 1994a; 1994b) • Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (Brown & Ryan, 2003) • Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale (Hayes & Feldman, 2004) • Response Styles Questionnaire (Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991) • Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (Hayes et al., 2004) • Langer Mindfulness Scale (Langer, 1999) • REFERENCES • Abela, J. R. Z., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2000). The hopelessness theory of depression: A test of the diathesis-stress component in the interpersonal and achievement domains. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 361-378. • Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review, 96, 358-372. • Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49-74. • Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Whitehouse, W. G., Hogan, M. E., Tashman, N. A., Steinberg, D. L., Rose, D. T., & Donovan, P. (1999). Depressogenic cognitive styles: Predictive validity, information processing and personality characteristics, and developmental origins. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 503-531. • Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D. A., & Taylor, G. J. (1994a). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale: I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 23-32. • Bagby, R. M., Taylor, G. J., & Parker, J. D. A. (1994b). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale: II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 33-40. • Brown, K. W. & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well- being. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 84, 822-848. • Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159. • Fresco, D. M. & Craighead, L. W. (2003). Explanatory flexibility: A cognitive process counterpart to explanatory style. Manuscript under review. • Fresco, D. M., Mennin, D. S., Turk, C. L., Dangelmaier, R., & Heimberg, R. G. (2000, November). The relationship of explanatory flexibility to emotion dysregulation: Towards an understanding of the benefits of flexibility. A poster presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy, New Orleans, LA. • Fresco, D. M. & Seligman, M. E. P. (2003). Association of explanatory flexibility to well-being: A secondary analysis of the PENN APEX study. Unpublished manuscript. Kent State University, Kent, OH. • Hayes, A. M. & Feldman, G. (2004). Clarifying the construct of mindfulness in the context of emotion regulation and the process of change in therapy. Clinical Psychology: Science & Practice, 11, 255-262. • Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T., Pistorello, J., Toarmino, D., Polusny, M., A., Dykstra, T. A., Batten, S. V., Bergan, J., Stewart, S. H., Zvolensky, M. J., Eifert, G. H., Bond, F. W., Forsyth J. P., Karekla, M., & McCurry, S. M. (2004). Measuring experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a working model. Manuscript under review. • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115-121. • Peterson, C., Semmel, A., Von Baeyer, C., Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1982). The Attributional Style Questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6, 287-300. • Seligman, M. E. P. (1974). Depression and learned helplessness. In R. J. Friedman & M.M. Katz (Eds.), The psychology of depression: Contemporary theory and research. Washington, D. C.: Winston- Wiley. • Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1991). The Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript, University of Iowa, Iowa City. • Williams, K. E., Chambless, D. L., & Ahrens, A. (1997). Are emotions frightening? An extension of the fear of fear construct. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 239-248. Fig. 2

More Related