1 / 1

INTRODUCTION

I nvariance of symptoms of mood disorder in people with temporal lobe epilepsy ( TLE ) Stephen Bowden, Rachel Reilly, Fiona Bardenhagen and Mark Cook . University of Melbourne & St. Vincent’s Hospital, Melbourne. INTRODUCTION

knut
Download Presentation

INTRODUCTION

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Invariance of symptoms of mood disorder in people with temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE)Stephen Bowden, Rachel Reilly,Fiona Bardenhagenand Mark Cook.University of Melbourne & St. Vincent’s Hospital, Melbourne. INTRODUCTION To fully represent a theoretical or latent-trait structure of behaviour, it is necessary to model observed variable intercepts and factor means in addition to the familiar factor loadings, factor variances and covariances, and residual variances (Meredith, 1993; Widaman & Reise, 1997). Represented in this way, it is possible to fully examine measurement model invariance across groups. The centrality of assumptions of invariance for fairness in testing, and theoretical equivalence of validity patterns are widely acknowledged (Byrne et al., 1988; Meredith, 1993). Since identification of clinical disorders usually involves interpretations of patterns of means or patterns of validity correlations, it is important to examine the assumptions of invariance. Widaman and Reise (1997) have defined several levels of invariance. Configural invariance involves the finding of a similar pattern of factors and indicator-variable assignments. Configural invariance is seen in the type of factor-analysis replication commonly reported in the clinical literature and, when reported from different samples, indicates a similar, but not necessarily identical pattern of latent variables structure. In contrast metric invariance involves successively more restrictive assumptions about the measurement model, (i) weak metricinvariance assumes identical factor loadings, (ii) strong metric invariance assumes identical observed variable intercepts, and (iii) strict metric invariance assumes identical reliabilities, across groups. “If strong [metric] invariance holds, group differences in both means and variances on the latent variables, which represent the constructs in psychological theories, are reflected in group differences in means and variances on the measured variables (Widaman & Reise, 1997, p. 295). In clinical evaluation of psychological adjustment amongst people diagnosed with TLE there is much speculation about the meaning of psychological symptoms. Some have suggested that symptoms of distress and poor adjustment should be interpreted as features of personality and psychopathology specific to people with TLE (Blumer, 1999). In contrast, others have suggested that exotic formulations of psychopathology distract from recognition and treatment of familiar psychological disorders, commonly seen in many people with significant disability (Devinsky, & Souhel, 1999). Central to the debate are symptoms of depression. If symptoms of mood disorder in people with TLE reflect behaviours specific to this clinical population, then it is reasonable to expect that elements of a measurement model of depression should differ from the model of depressive symptoms seen in other samples. In contrast, if we were to observe invariance of depressive symptoms across samples, then this observation would provide compelling evidence for the assumption that symptoms of mood disorder in people with TLE should be interpreted and treated as depression. RESULTS Data analysis: Scores on the BDI in both samples were subjected to maximum-likelihood confirmatory analysis using Lisrel 8.12 replicating, in both sample, the oblique three-factor solution reported by Byrne (1998), which measure the latent variables of (i) Negative Affect, (ii) Work/Performance Inhibition, and (iii) Somatic Elements. When this three-factor baseline model was analysed simultaneously in both groups, using the same method of model identification to that reported by Widaman & Reise (1997, p. 305) configural invariance was observed and the resulting fit statistics are reported in Table 1 (Model 1). Next, invariance constraints were placed on the factor-loading (Λ) matrix, the variable intercept (τ) vector, and the error variance matrix (Θ), to test the weak, strong, and strict invariance models (Models 2-4), respectively. The increments in χ2 and values of other goodness of fit statistics reported in Table 1, suggest that strong invariance obtains, and strict invariance is a plausible assumption. _______________________________________________________________________________________________ Table 1 Invariance Model χ² df Δχ² ΔdF RMSEA SRMR CAIC TLI CFI _______________________________________________________________________________________________ 1 Baseline 619.41 372 .045 .066 1519 .86 .87 (configural invariance) 2 Weak 642.43 390 23.02 18 .044 .075 1419 .86 .87 (Model 1 and Λ invariant) 3 Strong 669.62 408 27 19 18 .044 .075 1324 .86 .87 (Model 2 and τ invariant) 4 Strict 708.78 429 39.16 21 .044 .079 1220 .86 .86 (Model 3 and Θ invariant) ________________________________________________________________________________________________ DISCUSSION The results of the present study show that a well validated model of depressive symptoms is invariant across samples of patients with TLE and heterogeneous neurological disorders. Initial modeling (Model 1), showed configural invariance across samples, which implies that the same latent variable structure is measured by an identical pattern of items from the BDI. In other words, it can be assumed that the BDI measures a set of depressive symptoms in people with TLE, which is the same as that observed in our neurological reference group, and has been observed in numerous other samples (Byrne, 1998). In addition, the measurement model displayed precise evidence of strong metric invariance (Model 3), which implies that the same set of latent variables is measured across groups, with the same metric. The assumption of strong metric invariance is necessary for the uncomplicated (scale equivalent) interpretation of patterns of elevated scores, and external validity patterns (Widaman & Reise, 1997). Overall, this pattern of strong factorial invariance provides a precise test of the assumption that the behaviours underlying scores on the BDI are the same in people with TLE, as they are in people with other neurological conditions, and numerous other community samples (Byrne, 1998). The results suggest that exotic formulations of personality structure and depressive symptoms in people with TLE should be avoided, and the symptoms of mood disturbance should be treated in the same way as in other clinical populations, namely, as potential markers of depression. Strong validity data, of the kind reported in this study, should sensitise clinicians to the presence of mood disturbance in patients with organic conditions. METHOD Samples:Two samples were employed for this study, the first comprising 187 consecutive patients undergoing video-telemetry at St. Vincent’s Hospital Melbourne, for the investigation of TLE. The second sample comprised 150 consecutive patients admitted to the Clinical Neurosciences Department for investigation and treatment of heterogeneous neurological disorders, but not seizure disorders. The sample of patients with TLE comprised 94 females and 93 males, with a mean age of 35.6. The neurological sample comprised 59 females and 91 males, with a mean age of 46.0. Measure: Depressive symptoms were measured with the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), an instrument which is based on a well-validated clinical model of depression (Byrne, 1998; Tanaka & Huba, 1984). Mean BDI score for the TLE sample was 10.6, and for the neurological sample 12.8. Address for correspondence: s.bowden@psych.unimelb.edu.au REFERENCES Blumer, D. (1999). Devinsky, O., Souhel, N. (1999). Evidence supporting the temporal lobe epilepsy personality syndrome. Neurology, 53, S9-12. Byrne, B.M. (1998). Structural equation modelling with LISREL, PRELIS, and SIMPLIS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Devinsky, O., Souhel, N. (1999). Evidence against the existence of a temporal lobe epilepsy personality syndrome. Neurology 53, S13-25. Meredith, W. (1993). Measurement invariance, factor analysis and factorial invariance. Psychometrika, 58, 525-543. Tanaka, JS., & Huba, G.J. (1984). Confirmatory hierarchical factor analysis of psychological distress measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 621-635. Widaman, KF., Reise, SP. (1997). Exploring the measurement invariance of psychological instruments: Applications in the substance use domain. In Bryant, KJ. et al., (Eds). The science of prevention: Methodological advances from alcohol and substance abuse research. (pp. 281-324). Washington, DC, American Psychological Association.

More Related