1 / 33

Rediscovering the Role of Noncognitive Elements of Human Capital

Rediscovering the Role of Noncognitive Elements of Human Capital. Jerry Miner Seminar, Syracuse University 5 April 2012 Henry M. Levin. Non-Cognitive. Non-cognitive refers to: Attitudes and values. Social and emotional behavior. Cognitive refers to: Knowledge and its application.

kail
Download Presentation

Rediscovering the Role of Noncognitive Elements of Human Capital

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Rediscovering the Role of Noncognitive Elements of Human Capital Jerry Miner Seminar, Syracuse University 5 April 2012 Henry M. Levin

  2. Non-Cognitive Non-cognitive refers to: • Attitudes and values. • Social and emotional behavior. Cognitive refers to: • Knowledge and its application. • Cognitive and non-cognitive may interact as in process of problem-solving.

  3. Dominance of Cognitive Skills in Economic Explanations • Nothing inherent in G Becker or T W Schultz that limits human capital to cognitive skills. • Becker highlights the influence of families on the “knowledge, skills, values, and habits” of children. Human Capital (1964):21. • Can capitalize the income stream of any human attribute (even physical characteristics) into a measure of human capital as implied by Irving Fisher’s broader framework in The Theory of Interest (1930).

  4. Economists Ignore Noncognitive View • Human Capital interpretation of link between education and earnings developed in early 1960’s as contained in educational attainment. • Rate of return on educational attainment as an unopened black box. • Opening of black box led to estimation of Educational Production Functions.

  5. 50 Years of Educational Production Functions • Virtually all production functions assumed that test scores are the objective function of schools. • All used cognitive measures of outcome with notable exception of Card and Krueger 1992 who used income and found “right signs” for school effects on income. • Cognitive exclusivity is summarized by the many articles showing league tables of educational production function results limited only to student achievement (Hanushek summaries).

  6. James Heckman Influence • In the last decade or so, Heckman has introduced non-cognitive variables into explanation of educational outcomes and income. • Psychologists and economists are working jointly to discover specific non-cognitive variables that are important for educational success and earnings.

  7. Purpose of Presentation • Emphasize importance of non-cognitive influences on school outcomes and life outcomes. • Urge greater balance in assessments between cognitive and non-cognitive outcome measures. • Rationale. • Evidence. • Next steps.

  8. Inkeles on Socialization of Competence • “Effective participation in a modern industrial and urban society requires certain levels of skill in the manipulation of language and other symbol systems, such as arithmetic and time; the ability to comprehend and complete forms; information as to when and where to go for what; skills in interpersonal relations which permit negotiation, insure protection of one’s interests, and provide maintenance of stable and satisfying relations with intimates, peers, and authorities; motives to achieve, to master, to persevere; defenses to control and channel acceptably the impulses to aggression, to sexual expression, to extreme dependency, a cognitive style which permits thinking in concrete terms while still permitting reasonable handling of abstractions and general concepts; a mind which does not insist on excessively premature closure, is tolerant of diversity, and has some components of flexibility; a conative style which facilitates reasonably regular, steady, and persistent effort, relieved by rest and relaxation but not requiring long periods of total withdrawal or depressive psychic slump; and a style of expressing affect which encourages stable and enduring relationships without excessive narcissistic dependence or explosive aggression in the face of petty frustration. This is already a long list and surely much more could be added (Inkeles 1966: 280-1).

  9. Inkeles and Smith • Index of Modern Attitudes • Six countries (Argentina, Bangladesh, Chile, India, Israel, and Nigeria) n = almost 6.000 • Explanatory variables: education, work experience, mass media, consumer goods, father’s education, urbanization, skill level, length of urban residence, modernity of workplace, home and school; and schooling. • Explained 32-62 percent of variance in modernism. • Education was most powerful influence—2-3 times more powerful than any other. (standard regression coefficients).

  10. Why favor cognitive? • Long history of cognitive testing with relatively clear constructs, measures, statistical properties. • Widespread adoption. • Skills logically assumed to be cognitive. • (At the time of HC development, economists did not associate with psychologists. That has changed today.)

  11. Non-cognitive Ambiguity • Distinction not clear—interacts with cognitive. • Constructs and measurement are contested. • Labels differ: Social and Emotional (SEL). Social and Behavioral Attitudes and Values

  12. Early non-cognitive Interest • Coleman (1966) included efficacy, motivation, and other non-cognitive variables—didn’t use them. • Bowles and Gintis (1976) and Gintis (1971) argued that schools mainly emphasize non-cognitive development. • Rejected by most economists.

  13. Cognitive Test Scores or Behavioral Socialization • If cognitive skills explain effect schooling effect on earnings in human capital equations, addition of test scores should deflate severely the earnings coefficient for schooling. • Schooling variable is highly errorful (no adjustment for quality, effort, area of study, retrospective errors). • 25 studies with 58 estimates over four decades. • School coefficient retains about 82 percent of its value after test scores are added to the equation.

  14. Has effect of cognitive test score increased? • Murnane, Willett, Levy (1995) compared test score impacts on earnings between 1978 & 1986 and found rise in hourly wages related to math scores. • Bowles, Gintis,Osborne (2001) reviewed 65 estimates from 24 studies over 30 years and found no rise. • UK study for 1995-2004 also finds no increase in returns to cognitive skills.

  15. Other evidence • Teenage cognitive skills in predicting later earnings (Murnane et al. 2000). • Murnane et al. equations including test scores explain no more than 17 percent of variance. • NRC study of supervisor ratings of worker productivity found that only about 6 percent of variance was explained.

  16. Employer Needs • NRC study of High School Graduates by Employer Panel (1984) • Examined Cognitive needs, but also behavioral and social. • Interacting in socially appropriate manner; respect for opinions, customs, and individual differences of others. • Handling conflict maturely; participation reaching group decisions.

  17. NRC study continued. • Positive attitude toward self. • Self-discipline. • Regular attendance, punctuality, dependability. • Ability to set goals and allocate time to achievement of them. • Capacity to accept responsibility.

  18. Employer Employment Survey (1995) U.S. Dept. Ed. • 4,000 employers • Recruitment characteristics for hiring. (1-5 high) • Applicant attitude (4.6); communication skills (4.2). • Test scores, academic grades, reputation of applicants school (2.4 or 2.5).

  19. Independent Effects of non-cognitive (Perry Preschool) • Perry preschool—initial test score advantage faded in early grades. • But assessment at age 40 showed: earnings advantage of one third; half the crime conviction rate. • Higher graduation rates and more positive attitudes towards schools. • Something other than academic achievement must explain.

  20. GED--Heckman • GED recipients had similar test results as regular high school graduates who do not enroll in college. • GED earnings well below high school graduates and even lower than dropouts with similar test scores. • Ultimate educational attainments lower than for dropouts who do not take GED. • Something else happening—lower non-cognitive skills that affect employment.

  21. Tennessee Class Size Experiment • Dramatic reductions in class size for 1-4 years, K-3. • Modest increases in achievement. • Dramatic increases in high school graduation rates. (disadvantaged from 70 percent (no reduction of class size to 88 percent for 4 years of early class size reductions). • Well beyond the impact of prediction from early achievement advantage.

  22. Can non-cognitive be taught? • Experimental study of teaching of executive function (self-regulation)—more important than cognitive measures for school readiness. • Experimental study of Tools of the Mind shows dramatic effects on EF. (Barnett 2011) • Bandura cites impressive data-base that shows self-efficacy can be taught.

  23. Early Childhood Education Summary • Nores and Barnett (2010)—38 studies reviewing 30 pre-school interventions in 23 countries RFT or quasi-experiments. Found both cognitive and non-cognitive benefits. • Camilli et al (2010) meta-analysi of 123 experimental preschool studies showed effects on cognitive skills, student social skills and school progress.

  24. Durlak (2011) Social-Emotional Learning • 213 school based programs, experimental or quasi-experimental. 270,000 children 5-18. • Non-cognitive (SEL) interventions. • Effect sizes: social & emotional skills, .57; attitudes towards self and others, .23; positive social behaviors, .24; problems of conduct, .22; emotional distress, .24; and academic performance, .27, rising to .32 for longer term studies. • Raise student achievement by 11 percentiles. • Equivalent to closing the gap between U.S. and Canada and rise in rankings from 17th to 5th place.

  25. Heckman and Colleagues • Use longitudinal (NLSY) to analyze students educational investment on earnings and graduation. • Creates battery of non-cognitive scores from anti-social construct using student anxiety, headstrongness, hyperactivity, peer conflict along with cognitive test scores. • Models developmental path and impacts of investment in cognitive and non-cognitive skill on high school graduation and earnings. Impact of the two kinds of investments shift from cognitive at younger ages 6-9 to older ages 9-13.

  26. Swedish Study (Lindqvist, E. & Vestman, R. (2011) “The Labor Market Returns to Cognitive and Noncognitive Ability: Evidence from the Swedish Enlistment,” American Economic Review: Applied Economics, (3), 101-128 • Sample: 14703 men distributed evenly over 1965-74 birth. • Universal enlistment age 18 males. • Evaluation of cognitive ability (test) and noncognitive ability 70-80 item questionnaire and interview by psychologist. • Wages age 31-40.

  27. Swedish (cont.) • Non-cognitive effects on wages and earnings are higher than cognitive for a range of specifications. • Non-cognitive more important at wages and earnings below median and cognitive above median. • Change in 1 sd in predictor: • Non-cognitive ES = .33; Cognitive ES = .15 for wages. • Employment Status: 3.3% to 1.1%. • More measurement error in non-cognitive than cognitive measures.

  28. Five Factor Model • For last two decades considered as the “big five” on the basis of many studies. • Openness—inventive and curious as opposed to consistent and cautious. • Conscientiousness—efficient and organized as opposed to easy-going and careless. • Extraversion—outgoing and energetic as opposed to solitary and reserved. • Agreeableness—friendly and compassionate as opposed to cold and unkind. • Neuroticism—sensitive and nervous as opposed to secure and confident.

  29. Examples of big five findings. • Major study finds openness predicts SAT. • Agreeableness and conscientiousness predicted Peer ratings of team member performance beyond controls for specific skills and general cognitive ability. • Kyllonen and colleagues go well-beyond big five in their extensive study of noncognitive constructs and measures.

  30. Most Comprehensive Review • Almlund, M., Duckworth, A. L., Heckman, J. & Kautz, T. (2011) Personality Psychology and • Economics, (January 17, 2001 version) ¨IZA Workshop: Cognitive and Non-Cognitive • Skills¨, January 25-27, (Bonn, Germany). Available as: • http://iza.org/conference_files/CoNoCoSk2011/heckman_j130.pdf.

  31. Conclusions • Non-cognitive skills are important contributors to educational outcomes and life outcomes. • Can be altered by educational interventions. • Contribute to cognitive outcomes. • Impact of test scores is overstated if noncognitive measures are omitted from analysis (omitted variable bias).

  32. Implications • Ignoring role of non-cognitive skills as mediating and outcome variables leads to misleading educational policies. • NCLB with its focus on test scores only. • Basing school and teacher evaluations only on value-added in test scores provides only limited picture of school and teacher effects.

  33. Next Steps • Choose most promising non-cognitive constructs and measures. • Test these in structural models of learning outcomes and life outcomes. • Non-cognitive outcomes affect cognitive outcomes and possibly reverse—need to evaluate this. • Start to incorporate these into national and international assessments of learning and impacts of educational systems.

More Related