1 / 58

Development of Folk Knowledge in Psychology

This chapter explores the development of folk knowledge in children, including theories of mind and understanding others' beliefs and intentions. It discusses the integration of innate knowledge with constructivism and the skills underlying theory of mind.

gilll
Download Presentation

Development of Folk Knowledge in Psychology

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 1PSYCHOLOGY 3050:The Development of Folk Knowledge (Ch 6) Dr. Jamie Drover SN-3094, 864-8383 e-mail -- jrdrover@mun.ca Fall Semester, 2012

  2. Theory Theories • We have innate theories that we modify during childhood. • A theory is tested and then revised when it no longer explains new data. • We have innate knowledge, or processing constraints in certain domains. • Neonativism

  3. Theory Theories • They integrate innate knowledge with constructivism. • The cognitive processes that undergo gradual constructivist development are actually innate. • They require specific motor and/or sensory skills. • Object permanence • Very young infants can show this ability when tasks are modified.

  4. Theory Theories • Gopnik and Meltzoff propose that children are born with sets of rules for operating on particular representations. • These rules are altered by experience. • Development is still constructive. • The child has a particular theory. • There is disorganization. • A new coherent theory emerges.

  5. Developing a Theory of Mind • Recognizing different categories of mind, such as dreams, memories, beliefs, etc. • Includes the ability to understand that other people have beliefs, desires and intentions that are different from one's own. • A theory to understand, predict, and explain behavior.

  6. Development of Theory of Mind • Adult theory of mind is based on belief-desire reasoning. • We explain and predict what people do based on what we understand their beliefs and desires to be. • Referring to wants, wishes, hopes, and goals, and to their ideas, opinions and knowledge.

  7. Skills Underlying Theory of Mind • Viewing oneself and others as intentional agents. • Individuals who cause things to happen and whose behavior is designed to achieve some goal. • The ability to take the perspective of another person, i.e., understand the intentions of others.

  8. Skills Underlying Theory of Mind • This knowledge develops over the first few years of life, beginning with shared attention. • A triadic interaction between two partners and a third object. • Appears at about 9 months. • At 12 months, they point to things that others aren’t aware of. • At 12 to 18 months, they use the gaze of others to achieve shared attention.

  9. Skills Underlying Theory of Mind • There is early evidence that infants and toddlers view others as intentional agents. • More likely to copy the behavior of a model who is engaged in the action on purpose. • See Carpenter et al (1998) p 202. • See Meltzoff (1995) p 202.

  10. The Development of Mind Reading • Researchers have developed several tasks to assess when and how children develop this latter aspect of theory of mind. • False-belief task: a child watches as a candy is hidden in a special location. • Another child is present when the candy is hidden. • The hidden candy is then relocated.

  11. The Development of Mind Reading • Child is asked whether the other child will know the location of the candy. • 4-year-olds can solve the problem. 3-year-olds can not. • A variation of this is the Smarties task. • Suggests that children are unable to remember their original belief.

  12. The Development of Mind Reading • Young children have difficulty with contradictory evidence. • Can’t deal with two representations of a single object simultaneously. • Similar to the dual-representation hypothesis. • Young children will fail in situations where they must consider two beliefs or representations for a single target.

  13. The Development of Mind Reading • Gopnik and Astington assessed 3, 4, and 5 year-old’s abilities to solve tasks that require children to deal with contradictory evidence (See Fig 6-2 p 204). • Performance varied with age. • A domain-general mechanism underlies representational abilities.

  14. The Development of Mind Reading • Young children lack executive function. • Cognitive abilities involved in planning, executing, and inhibiting actions. • In theory of mind tasks, children often have to inhibit a dominant response to pass the task. • See the mean monkey example (p. 205). • Note however, 3-year-olds can solve other tasks that require an understanding of other’s minds.

  15. The Development of Mind Reading • See p. 206 (Clemens & Perner, 1994) • Implicitly, children were correct. • 3-year-olds have an implicit understanding of false belief that exceeds their verbal understanding. • In some studies, 18-month-olds show the beginnings of theory of mind. • See page 207 (Repacholi & Gopnik, 1997).

  16. The Development of Mind Reading • Performance on false beliefs task are related to family size. • Children from larger families perform better. • This only helps if one has older siblings. • Important for children with low language skills.

  17. Deception • When children play tricks on others, they seem to be aware of what the other person does and does not know. • 2- and 3-year-olds are often capable of deception. • See p. 210 (Chandler et al. 1989) • 2.5 to 4 year-old children showed several types of deception. • Withhold evidence, destroy evidence, lying, producing false evidence.

  18. Hiding Containers Sponge Footprints Main Container

  19. Deception • Even 2.5-3 year-olds will engage in deceptive practices to instill false beliefs in others. • See page 255 (Hala & Chandler, 1996; Sullivan & Winner, 1993).

  20. Do 3-Year-Olds Have a Theory of Mind • Currently there is debate whether 3-year-olds possess theory of mind. • Most researchers agree that 3-year-olds appear to have a limited knowledge of other’s minds. • Some argue that 3-year-olds have competence, but the nature of the task prevents them from showing it. • Others argue that there is real conceptual change during the preschool years.

  21. Do 3-Year-Olds Have a Theory of Mind • Wellman et al. (2001) conducted a meta-analysis of 178 studies. • They found age effects which support the conceptual change position.

  22. Theory of Mind, Evolved Modules, and Autism • Some theorists believe that Theory of Mind evolved during the course of human evolution and is the basis of social intelligence. • It’s assumed that we have domain-specific modules to handle theory of mind. • According to Baron-Cohen (1995), there are four interacting modules involved in mindreading that develop in the first 4 years.

  23. Theory of Mind, Evolved Modules, and Autism • Intentionality Detector (ID): interprets moving objects as having some intention • Eye Direction Detector (EDD): Detects the presence of eyes. Determines when another organism is looking at them. Develops between birth and 9 months. • Shared Attention Mechanism: Involves three-way interactions. Develops from 9-18 months.

  24. Theory of Mind, Evolved Modules, and Autism • Theory of Mind Module: Understanding that others have different beliefs, desires, and intentions (develops between 18 and 48 months).

  25. The Empathizing System • Baron-Cohen proposed The Emotion Detector (TED) which develops by 9 months of age. • It can represent affective or emotional states between two people. • Within 6 months of life, infants can pick up on the emotions of others. • At 9 months, the info derived from TED can be converted into a triadic representation of the SAM.

  26. The Empathizing System • The Empathizing SyStem (TESS) is online at 14 months of age. • It permits an empathic reaction to another person’s emotions, and assumes there is an associated drive to help others.

  27. Mindblindness • The inability to read minds. • Advanced forms of mindreading and empathizing are absent or delayed in children with autism. • Autistic children fail false-belief and theory of mind tasks but can pass nonsocial tasks. • They perform well on tasks requiring the ID and EDD modules, but not those requiring SAM or Theory of mind.

  28. Mindblindness • Children with Down Syndrome perform well on theory-of-mind tasks, but perform poorly on tasks of intelligence.

  29. The Development of Spatial Cognition • Processing information with respect to their spatial relations. • Coding information about the environment. • Knowing where something is in relation to you, or in relation to other objects and locations. • See Table 6.4

  30. Spatial Orientation • How people understand the placement of objects in space with themselves as a reference point. • Eg. Distinguishing geographic directions in an unfamiliar locale, drawing your way from point A to B on a map. • Relatively well-developed in preschool. • See Huttenlocher & Vasilyeva (2003) on p 227 • See Newcombe et al. (1999) on p 227

  31. Spatial Orientation • Older children are even more advanced…can form cognitive maps. • i.e., 5, 7, and 10-year-olds could re-create the layout of a large model town based on memory (Herman & Siegel, 1978). • Got better with age. • They get better at using real maps over the school years. • Adultlike by age 10

  32. Spatial Orientation • Uttal et al. (2001) showed 3, 4, and 5-year-olds yellow carpet with 27 cups on it. • They had to find stickers under some cups using a map. • The maps included lines for half the kids, no lines for the other half. • Children got better with age. • Lines improved the performance for 5-year-olds.

  33. Spatial Orientation • Maps are a tool of intellectual adaptation that can eventually be internalized. • There is a reciprocal relationship between maps and spatial cognition. • Uttal & Wellman (1989) had 4 to 7-year-old children learn which of 6 animals went into each of 6 rooms in a life-sized playhouse. • Children learned the locations based on a map or on flash cards.

  34. Spatial Orientation • The children who learned with the map performed better on a “walk-through.” • The experience with the map allowed them to more easily see spatial relationships among elements.

  35. Spatial Visualization • Involves visual/mental operations. • Often assessed using mental rotation. • A stimulus must be rotated to see if it matches another. • Even 4 and 5-year-olds can do this. • Adults can have problems with complex stimuli.

  36. Spatial Visualization

  37. Spatial Visualization • Piaget and Inhelder (1967) tested spatial visualization using the water-level problem. • Most children can do this by age 7.

  38. Object and Location Memory • Remembering objects and their positions. • Often tested using card games. • Young children (5 yrs) perform almost as well as adults early in these games. • Strategies are less important.

  39. Sex Differences in Spatial Cognition • There are sex differences in spatial cognition that may have been selected for through the course of evolution. • Males needed to develop spatial abilities in order to navigate (Geary, 2007). • Sex differences in map reading and mental rotation have been found in preschool years. • Mental rotation differences may exist in infancy.

  40. Sex Differences in Spatial Cognition • According to meta-analyses, the magnitude of sex differences is very small. • Only 1 to 5% of the difference is due to gender. • Mental rotation is especially prominent. • Females show better performance in object and location memory. • May be due to evolution…role as gatherers. • Must be able to perceive small stimulus differences.

  41. Sex Differences in Spatial Cognition • Differential experiences may also play a role. • Newcombe et al. (1983) asked college students to classify activities as masculine, feminine, or neutral (see Table 6-5). • Tasks with high spatial content were considered masculine. • They found a gender difference on a test with strong spatial components.

  42. Sex Differences in Spatial Cognition • The more spatial activities one engages in, the greater one’s spatial ability.

  43. Object-Oriented Play and Tool Use • Object-Oriented Play: manipulation of objects (banging or throwing), including the use of objects to build something. • Difficult to distinguish from exploration. • Often involves making noise.

  44. Object-Oriented Play and Tool Use • Play eventually becomes more sophisticated. • Building things --- boys more likely. • May be a biological origin.

  45. Object-Oriented Play and Tool Use • Preschoolers spend 10 to 15% of their time engaged in object play. • This object play allows children to learn how objects can be used as tools. • This is something very few species can do.

  46. Learning to use Tools • Common in problem solving: forks, pens, hammers, etc. • Non-humans also use tools to solve problems

  47. Learning to use Tools • May originate from infants’ manipulation of their physical world (Lockman, 2000) • It’s a gradual process of discovery that arises from infants’ and children’s interaction with objects in the real world to achieve a perceptual outcome.

  48. The Development of Tool Use in Young Children • Piaget speculated that infants could use tools by their first birthday. • 9- to 12 mo-old infants learn to solve lure-retrieval problems • E.g., can use a stick to reach toy outside playpen • Chen and Siegler (2000): systematic study of tool use in toddlers: 18- to 30-mo • Child seated across from out-of-reach toys • Several tools available • Only one will retrieve the toy • Three trials – encouraged to get toy • Hint and modeling conditions given

  49. The Development of Tool Use in Young Children

  50. The Development of Tool Use in Young Children

More Related