1 / 17

The longterm consequences of malnutrition on children: New evidence from Guatemala

The longterm consequences of malnutrition on children: New evidence from Guatemala. John Hoddinott International Food Policy Research Institute April 2009. Introduction.

etenia
Download Presentation

The longterm consequences of malnutrition on children: New evidence from Guatemala

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. The longterm consequences of malnutrition on children: New evidence from Guatemala John Hoddinott International Food Policy Research Institute April 2009

  2. Introduction • Improving the nutritional status of pre-school children – in terms better anthropometric outcomes and reduced prevalence of micro-nutrient deficiencies – is receiving increased attention from the World Bank, bilateral donors and some developing country governments: • The World Bank’s “Repositioning nutrition as central to development” • Two rationales are given for this renewed emphasis on improving nutrition: • Its intrinsic value– Being well nourished is a “good thing” • Its instrumental value - Malnutrition has high costs in terms of poor health, cognitive development and loss of earnings Page 2

  3. Introduction • While there is a growing body of evidence of the instrumental value of nutrition, much of it discussed in “Repositioning …”, this evidence suffers from two significant weaknesses: • Many of the benefits are measured over a relatively short time horizon. From these studies, it is not clear if the beneficial effects of improving dimensions of nutrition are sustained or if they “fade out” over time. • Much of the evidence on the economic benefits is indirect. For example: • We observe a causal relation between improved nutrition and some dimension of human capital, say cognitive skills • We assume that this causal relationship will persist • We draw evidence from the extant literature on the relationship between cognitive skills and some economic outcome, say wages. • We assume that this relationship is applicable to the setting in which the improved nutrition has taken place • We adduce a relationship between improved nutrition and this economic outcome Page 3

  4. Introduction • In this presentation, I present new evidence on the instrumental value of investing in nutrition. This is taken from a long term, longitudinal study of a nutrition supplementation trial that was fielded in rural Guatemala. • Using the data collected during the trial and afterwards, I will discuss: • The impact of one dimension of malnutrition, stunting, on wages in adulthood • The pathways by which this impact occurs and • The long term effects of the intervention itself • In doing so, I draw on ongoing and published work with my collaborators, particularly Jere Behrman, John Maluccio, Reynaldo Martorell and Raphael Flores Page 4

  5. Guatemala Study

  6. Guatemala Study • Two matched-pair sets of villages (small & large) • Randomization into: • Atole– high protein, high calorie, micronutrients • Fresco – no protein, low calorie, micronutrients • Centrally located feeding center open at convenient hours, on-demand service • Free preventative healthcare • Enrolled all 0-7 year olds in 1969, all new births included until after supplementation ended in 1977 (n=2392) • Extensive data collected on anthropometric status Page 6

  7. Guatemala Study • In 2002-04, these individuals re-surveyed. • Of the 2392 original participants • Excluded deceased (11%), international migrants (7%) and untraceable (4%) • 1855 (78%): Targeted sample living in Guatemala in 2002 • 1471 (62% original; 79% targeted) were resurveyed • Correcting for potential attrition bias has no significant impacts on any of the results discussed here • Respondents were asked about: • Occupation, Income & wages • Underwent a physical examination; and • Completed tests on reading, vocabulary and nonverbal cognitive skills • Community level data was collected on school characteristics and changes in livelihoods Page 7

  8. Guatemala Study • The key feature of this study is that we have data on the following: • Nutritional status as a pre-schooler • Exposure to a randomized nutrition intervention • Family background and community characteristics • Height, measures of strength and other measures of health • Earnings in adulthood • Measures of completed schooling and selected cognitive skills in adulthood • Reading comprehension and vocabulary (Inter-American Reading test) and • Nonverbal cognitive skills (Raven’s Progressive Matrices) Page 8

  9. Results (1): What is the impact of stunting on adult productivity? • Modeling strategy: • Dependent variable is log of income earned per hour worked: • Income earned from wage labor, own business activities, own-farm production • KEY right hand side variable is dummy variable equaling one if individual was not stunted at age 72 months, zero if stunted. • Stunting is defined as having a height, standardized for age and sex, that is two standard deviations or more below the median height of a reference population • Being stunted is treated as endogenous using exposure to the intervention from 0-36 months, exposure from 0-36 months x atole , log mother’s height • We have excellent IV diagnostics • Other controls include sex, potential labor market experience, labor market conditions at age 15, school characteristics at age 7, family background Page 9

  10. What is the impact of stunting on adult productivity? • With full set of controls, not being stunted raises wages by approximately 31 percent • Effect is larger for men (57%) than for women (22%) • Effect is larger for individuals who were severely stunted (< 3 SD), 48% • We can compare the impacts of not being stunted with the impact of improving student-teacher ratios. Based on our preliminary estimates, it would take a 40% reduction in student-teacher ratios at age 7 to get the equivalent increase in earnings that we get when we “switch” a child from being stunted to “not stunted.” Page 10

  11. Results (2): What are the pathways by which pre-school nutrition affects wages? • The nutrition literature suggests two pathways that could generate these results: • Individuals who were stunted as pre-schoolers might have reduced cognitive skills in adulthood • Individuals who were stunted as pre-schoolers are shorter as adults • We can think of these pathways as education or cognition (“brains”) and height, health or strength (“brawn”) • Modeling strategy: • Dependent variable is log of income earned per hour worked: • KEY right hand side variables are measures of cognitive ability: • Completed grades of schooling, reading/vocabulary score, Raven’s progressive matrices • AND • Measures of strength: • Height, fat-free mass, hand grip strength, cardio-vascular endurance Page 11

  12. What are the pathways by which pre-school nutrition affects wages? • Cognitive skills - “brains” - have a large effect on earnings however measured. • For example, a one standard deviation increase on a reading/vocabulary test raises hourly returns to labor by about 40%. • By contrast, none of the measures of strength have a causal effect on earnings • This suggests that it is the impact of poor nutrition on the development of cognitive skills that is the driver of the economic case for investing in nutrition Page 12

  13. Results (3): Does malnutrition affect cognition abilities in adulthood? • Modeling strategy: • Dependent variable are two measures of cognitive abilities: • Reading/vocabulary score and Raven’s progressive matrices • KEY right hand side variable is dummy variable equaling one if individual was not stunted at age 72 months, zero if stunted. We also control for grades of completed schooling and number of years in skilled employment • ALL these variables are treated as endogenous using exposure to the intervention from 0-36 months, exposure from 0-36 months x atole , sex, being a twin, labor market conditions at age 18, school characteristics at age 6. Again we have excellent IV diagnostics • Not being stunted raises • Raven’s progressive matrices score by 0.7 standard deviations and • Reading z scores by approximately 0.5 standard deviations. This is equivalent, in terms of effect size, to increasing schooling attainment by four grades Page 13

  14. Results (4): Did the original intervention affect cognition abilities in adulthood? • Dependent variables are measures of cognitive abilities and outcomes: • Reading/vocabulary scores, Raven’s progressive matrices and grades of completed schooling • KEY right hand side variable is exposure to the intervention from 0-36 months x atole(an intent-to-treat model) • We also control for exposure to the intervention from 0-36 months, village dummy variables, family background and school characteristics • We find that exposure to atole from 0-36 months increases scores on reading/vocabulary and Raven’s progressive matrices by approximately 0.25 of a standard deviation. It increases female grade attainment by one full grade, but has no impact on male grade attainment Page 14

  15. Results (5): Did the original intervention affect wages and incomes in adulthood? • Dependent variable is log of income earned per hour worked: • KEY right hand side variable is exposure to the intervention from 0-36 months x atole(an intent-to-treat model) • We also control for exposure to the intervention from 0-36 months, village dummy variables, family background, school characteristics and labor market shocks • Exposure to atole before, but not after, age 3 years was associated with higher hourly wages, but only for men. For exposure to atole from 0 to 2 years, the increase was US$0·67 per hour which meant a 46% increase in average wages. There was a non-significant tendency for hours worked to be reduced and for annual Page 15

  16. Summary • This work, some of which is preliminary, provides evidence of the instrumental value of investments that improve the nutritional status of pre-school children by demonstrating: • A causal link between stunting and adult wages (result #1) • That the causal pathways by which this occurs are those that run from stunting to lowered cognitive skills to lower wages (results #2 and 3) • That interventions aimed at improving nutritional status can have persistent effects on cognitive skills and productivity (results #4 and 5) • As such, it reinforces the value of repositioning nutrition as being central to development Page 16

  17. References Result (1): “The impact of pre-school malnutrition on wages in adulthood”, in preparation. Result (2): J. Behrman, J. Hoddinott, J. Maluccio,R. Martorell, 2008, “Labor Market Returns to Intellectual and Physical Human Capital in a Poor Developing Country” in preparation. Result (3): J. Behrman, J. Hoddinott, J. Maluccio,E. Soler-Hampejsek, E. Behrman,R. Martorell,M. Ramírez-Zea, A. Stein, 2008, “What Determines Adult Cognitive Skills?Impacts of Pre-Schooling, Schooling and Post-Schooling Experiences in Guatemala” mimeo, International Food Policy Research Institute, Washington DC Result (4): J. Maluccio, J. Hoddinott, J. Behrman, R. Martorell, A. Quisumbing, and A. Stein, “The Impact of nutrition during early childhood on education among Guatemalan Adults”, Economic Journal, forthcoming. Result (5): J. Hoddinott, J. Maluccio, J. Behrman, R. Flores and R. Martorell, 2008, “Effect of a nutrition intervention during early childhood on economic productivity in Guatemalan adults”,The Lancet, vol. 371, pp. 411-416. Page 17

More Related