Psyc54 lecture on jan 18 2005 part i
This presentation is the property of its rightful owner.
Sponsored Links
1 / 37

PSYC54 Lecture on Jan. 18, 2005: Part I PowerPoint PPT Presentation

  • Uploaded on
  • Presentation posted in: General

PSYC54 Lecture on Jan. 18, 2005: Part I.

Download Presentation

PSYC54 Lecture on Jan. 18, 2005: Part I

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation

Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author.While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server.

- - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - E N D - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -

Presentation Transcript

Psyc54 lecture on jan 18 2005 part i

PSYC54 Lecture on Jan. 18, 2005: Part I

Please note: Other PSYC54 Lecture notes, including those for Jan. 25 (Chapter III), can be found on the table, click ‘Chapter X – Notes,’ X meaning the number of the chapter. The chapter contents are on

Color and Motion in

Shape-from-shadow Pictures

Humera Iqbal, John Kennedy, Juan Bai, & Tom Nolis

Tom nolis s colored shape from shadow study

Tom Nolis’s Colored Shape-from-shadow Study

  • Apparent luminance may not be the same as physical luminance, somehow affecting our performance in shape-from-shadow perception.

  • This may be especially true if the shadowed region is in one color (e.g., green), and the line at the shadow border is in another (e.g., red).

    • As another example, a blue line may be perceived as darker than a yellow shadow even though they have the same luminance scores in the software creating the image, and shape-from-shadow perception may be diminished.

Tactile pictures juan bai john kennedy

PSYC54 Lecture on Jan. 18, 2005: Part II

Tactile PicturesJuan Bai & John Kennedy

Rationale: Surfaces and edges are tangible as well as visible, hence outline drawings should be possible for the blindfolded sighted people as well as the blind, including the congenitally blind.

(Tactile pictures, or haptic pictures, are also called raised-line drawings.)

D angiulli kennedy heller 1998

D’Angiulli, Kennedy, & Heller (1998)

  • Participants:

    • blind children aged 8-13

    • blindfolded sighted children, same age group

  • Materials:

    • eight tactile pictures of common objects

D angiulli et al s 1998 procedure

D’Angiulli et al.’s (1998) Procedure

  • On trial 1:

  • The children identified the 8 tactile pictures one by one.

  • On trial 2:

  • They identified the same pictures, but in different orders.

D angiulli et al s 1998 accuracy results

D’Angiulli et al.’s (1998) Accuracy Results

Trial 1 Trial 2

  • Blind children 46% 56%

  • Blindfolded

    sighted children 9% 28%

Why accuracy of at most 56

Cup or Turtle?

Why Accuracy of at Most 56%?

  • The perceiver may find information about figure-ground relations, orientation, occlusion, the observer’s vantage point, and three dimensionality in tactile pictures very difficult to appreciate (Eriksson, 1998).

D angiulli et al s 1998 important findings

D’Angiulli et al.’s (1998) Important Findings

  • The children often knew where to correct themselves on trial 2:

    • The sighted children repeated 95% of the correct suggestions and only 3% of the incorrect ones.

    • The blind repeated 100% versus 24%.

  • They did so without any feedback.

Kennedy and bai s 2002 proposal

Kennedy and Bai’s (2002) Proposal

  • The children in D’Angiulli et al. (1998) might have a criterion to judge how well their suggestions matched the shapes in the pictures, and to decide whether to stay with them.

  • Kennedy and Bai (2002) called this criterion “fit.”

What is fit

What is “Fit?”

  • Fit is the degree of correspondence

    between the suggested name and the

    • shapes

    • proportions, and

    • orders

      of the parts in the picture.

Reasoning about fit

Reasoning about Fit

  • If most lines are apt for the suggested name, a person may give a high fit, and is more likely to stay with this name.

  • .

  • Otherwise, the person may give a low fit, and is more likely to change the name.

  • On average, correct names for tactile pictures should have higher fit with the pictures than incorrect names.

Reasoning about fit cont d

Reasoning about Fit (Cont’d)

  • Relatively high fit:

    Ss. are most likely to stay with the name

    on a later trial.

  • Relatively low fit:

    Ss. are most likely to change the name to another

    candidate on a later trial.

Fit for the names that stay should be higher than fit for the names that are changed.

Kennedy and bai 2002 exp 1 stimuli

Kennedy and Bai (2002) Exp. 1 Stimuli

Demonstration (optic and haptic): star

Practice (haptic): tree

Old/repeated (trials 1 and 2): fork, hammer, key, person, scissors, swan, toothbrush, umbrella

New (trial 2 only): car, sailboat

Exp 1 procedure

Exp. 1 Procedure

  • Twelve blindfolded sighted university students named 8 tactile pictures, each within 2 minutes.

They gave fit judgments for these suggestions, using a 7-point scale (1 means a very low fit, 7 a perfect fit).

After a break, they named the same pictures again and judged fit, but in different orders and mixed with 2 new pictures.

Exp 1 results on accuracy

Exp. 1 Results on Accuracy

  • Accuracy of the 8 suggestions on trial 1: 61%

  • Accuracy of the 8 suggestions on trial 2: 69%

    (71% for all 10, including ‘old’ and ‘new’).

Larger dimension means higher accuracy rate

Larger Dimension MeansHigher Accuracy Rate

  • The largest dimension of the pictures in K and B (2002) was 22.5 cm (accuracy > 60%):

    - 50% bigger than Lederman et al. (1990)

    (accuracy up to 33%).

    - 30% bigger than D’Angiulli et al. (1998)

    (accuracy up to 56%).

Exp 1 results on accuracy and fit

Exp. 1 Results on Accuracy and Fit

  • On trial 1,

    mean fit for the correct names: 5.6 (on the 7-point scale).

    mean fit for the incorrect names: 3.6

    (p < 0.001).

Exp 1 results on accuracy and fit cont d

Exp. 1 Results on Accuracy and Fit (Cont’d)

  • On trial 2, for the 8 old pictures,

    mean fit for the correct names: 5.8;

    mean fit for the incorrect names: 3.8

    (p < 0.001).

  • On trial 2, for all 10 pictures,

    mean fit for the correct names: 5.8;

    mean fit for the incorrect names: 3.7

    (p < 0.001).

Exp 1 results on repetition and fit

Exp. 1 Results on Repetition and Fit

  • Names that stayed on trial 2 had a mean fit of 5.3 on trial 1.

  • Names that were changed on trial 2 had a mean fit of 2.9 on trial 1

    (p < 0.02).

Exp 1 conclusions

Exp. 1 Conclusions

  • Correct names had higher fit scores than incorrect names. So fit judgments can predict accuracy for identification of tactile pictures.

  • Names with lower fit judgments were more likely to be changed than names with higher fit judgments. So fit might be the criteria the participates used to decide whether to stay with previous responses.

Exp 2 rationale

Exp. 2 Rationale

  • High-fit pictures might be remembered as a single chunk, efficiently, since details did not have to be memorized separately.

  • Ill-fitting pictures might have extra parts that presented difficulty in recognition memory.

  • It is likely that the more parts of the display corresponded to the suggested referent, the more it is accessible in our memory task. So higher fit judgments should be coupled with better memory of tactile pictures.

Exp 2 stimuli

Exp. 2 Stimuli

Demonstration (optic and haptic): apple

Practice (haptic): tree

Old/repeated (trials 1 and 2): cup, sailboat, table, telephone, plus the 8 stimuli used in Exp. 1 (all inverted on trial 2).

New (trial 2 only): star, triangle, car (turned 90-degree clockwise), house (turned 90-degree counterclockwise), coat hanger and pine tree (inverted).

Exp 2 procedure

Exp. 2 Procedure

  • Twelve blindfolded Ss. (= subjects) saw tactile pictures twice, the 2nd time mixed with new, distraction pictures. On trial 2 they were asked whether they saw each tactile picture on trial 1.

  • To increase the difficulty of the task:

    - twelve pictures on trial 1, 6 more on trial 2;

    - time allowed was 90s on trial 1, 45s on trial 2;

    - trial 2 introduced various orientations.

Psyc54 lecture on jan 18 2005 part i

Exp. 2 Results and Conclusion

  • Pictures successfully remembered as ‘old’ had a mean fit of 5.4 on trial 1.

  • Pictures failed to be recognized as ‘old’ had a mean fit of 2.5 on trial 1.

    (p < 0.001).

  • Therefore, fit can predict performance on recognition memory for tactile pictures.

Exp 3 procedure and findings

Exp. 3 Procedure and Findings

  • Another group of 12 blindfolded Ss. assessed fit of the suggestions offered by matched Exp. 1 Ss.

  • Results = mean Pearson correlation coefficient of the 12 pairs of Ss. was 0.6; all r’s were positive (p < .0001, two-tailed binomial).

    - Six of the r’s reached significant level beyond p = .05.

Exp 3 conclusions

Exp. 3 Conclusions

  • Touch is consistent across participants.

  • Fit judgment is based on physical correspondence between lines and possible referents. It is an objective matter.

Exp 4 procedure

Exp. 4 Procedure

  • Still another group of blindfolded Ss. each touched 2 pictures and judged fit for their names offered by matched Exp. 1 Ss.

    (These 2 pictures received the highest/lowest fit scores in Exp. 1.)

  • Then, they gave confidence rating on a 7-point scale for their fit judgments (1 low, 7 high).

Exp 4 findings

Exp. 4 Findings

  • The difference between fit judgments for the 2 pictures per participant was 3.0, twice that of the difference between the confidence judgments, p < .007.

  • This is because both high and low fit judgments received high confidence scores, with mean confidence of 6.4 for high fit judgments (M = 6.6), and mean confidence of 4.9 for low fit judgments (M = 3.6).

Exp 4 conclusions

Exp. 4 Conclusions

  • Even low fit judgments can be coupled with relatively high confidence judgments, since poor fit is based on assessment of the correspondence between the physical features of the picture and the physical features of a suggested identity.

  • Fit judgments cannot be substituted by confidence judgments.

Exp 5 procedure and findings

Exp. 5 Procedure and Findings

  • A group of Ss. visually judged fit for the names offered by matched Exp. 1 Ss.

  • Results = fit judgments of the 2 groups

  • (visual and tactual group) were positively correlated.

  • Therefore, touch and vision extract

  • the same information from outlines.

Overall conclusions

Overall Conclusions

  • D’Angiulli et al.’s (1998) study indicated that Ss. have a accurate and consistent standard to judge their identification of tactile pictures. K and B’s (2002) experiments supported the idea that the basis for this standard is fit, which is the physical correspondence between the picture and its suggested name.

  • Fit judgments of tactile pictures are related usefully to identification accuracy, repetition, remembering, confidence, and visual judgments.

Psyc54 lecture on jan 18 2005 part i

Blind People and Outline Drawings

Lines copying surface edges give us surface edge impressions, including lines in tactile pictures.

Drawings identified by blind children (Kennedy, 1993)

Psyc54 lecture on jan 18 2005 part i

Kennedy et al.’s (1972) study on Tactile Pictures

Kennedy, Fox, and O’Grady’s (1972) raised-line drawings included four imprints and four projections

Psyc54 lecture on jan 18 2005 part i

Kennedy et al.’s (1972) Study

  • The stimuli were 8 tactile pictures measured about 10 cm by 7 cm.

  • Thirty-four sighted Harvard students were asked to identify the pictures, firstly by touch (i.e., blindfolded), then by vision.

  • Results = haptically identified 2.4 of the 8, the easiest being the hand, the hardest being man with crossed arms and man with arm up (one identification for each).

Psyc54 lecture on jan 18 2005 part i

Kennedy et al.’s (1972) Study (Cont’d)

  • A group of 8 blind Harvard students (5 congenitally blind) tried to identify the same 8 pictures by touch.

  • On average the blind identified 1.25 pictures.

    - easiest: hand (three times), fork, & cup.

  • Eight blind volunteers in Toronto, mostly teens, touched each picture for 2 minutes.

  • On average they identified 1.75 pictures.

    - easiest: hand, fork, & face (three for each).

Implications from kennedy et al 1972

Implications from Kennedy et al. (1972)

  • Errors made by the blind do not appear to be random, but make visual sense.

    - e.g., the table was called by the Toronto blind volunteers a house (twice); the fork was called a tree (twice), a brush, an ice-cream cone, etc.

  • There is some practical ability with pictures in the blind. On the other hand, the 10-20% accuracy rate calls for improvement. Perhaps providing a suitable context will do?

Psyc54 lecture on jan 18 2005 part i


D’Angiulli, A., Kennedy, J. M., & Heller, M. A. (1998). Blind children recognizing tactile pictures respond like sighted children given guidance in exploration. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 39, 187-190.

Eriksson, Y. (1988). Tactile Pictures: Pictorial Representations for the Blind 1784-1940. Gothenburg: Gothenburg University Press.

Kennedy, J. M. (1993). Drawing and the Blind. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Kennedy, J. M., & Bai, J. (2002). Haptic pictures: Fit judgments predict identification, recognition memory, and confidence. Perception, 31(8), 1013-1026.

Kennedy, J. M., Fox, N., & O’Grady, K. (1972). Can ‘haptic pictures help the blind see? Harvard Graduate School of Education Bulletin, 16, 22-23.

Lederman, S. J., Klatzky, R. L., Chataway, C., & Summers, C (1990). Visual mediation and the haptic recognition of two-dimensional pictures of common objects. Perception and Psychophysics, 47, 54-64.

  • Login