1 / 39

For Wednesday

For Wednesday. Read chapter 22, sections 4-6 Homework: Chapter 18, exercise 7. Program 4. Any questions?. Model Neuron (Linear Threshold Unit). Neuron modelled by a unit ( j ) connected by weights, w ji , to other units ( i ): Net input to a unit is defined as: net j = S w ji * o i

amber-lopez
Download Presentation

For Wednesday

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. For Wednesday • Read chapter 22, sections 4-6 • Homework: • Chapter 18, exercise 7

  2. Program 4 • Any questions?

  3. Model Neuron(Linear Threshold Unit) • Neuron modelled by a unit (j) connected by weights, wji, to other units (i): • Net input to a unit is defined as: netj = S wji * oi • Output of a unit is a threshold function on the net input: • 1 if netj > Tj • 0 otherwise

  4. Multi­Layer Neural Networks • Multi­layer networks can represent arbitrary functions, but building an effective learning method for such networks was thought to be difficult. • Generally networks are composed of an input layer, hidden layer, and output layer and activation feeds forward from input to output. • Patterns of activation are presented at the inputs and the resulting activation of the outputs is computed. • The values of the weights determine the function computed. • A network with one hidden layer with a sufficient number of units can represent any boolean function.

  5. Basic Problem • General approach to the learning algorithm is to apply gradient descent. • However, for the general case, we need to be able to differentiate the function computed by a unit and the standard threshold function is not differentiable at the threshold.

  6. Differentiable Threshold Unit • Need some sort of non­linear output function to allow computation of arbitary functions by mulit­layer networks (a multi­layer network of linear units can still only represent a linear function). • Solution: Use a nonlinear, differentiable output function such as the sigmoid or logistic function oj = 1/(1 + e-(netj - Tj) ) • Can also use other functions such as tanh or a Gaussian.

  7. Error Measure • Since there are mulitple continuous outputs, we can define an overall error measure: E(W) = 1/2 *( S S(tkd - okd)2) dD kK where D is the set of training examples, K is the set of output units, tkd is the target output for the kth unit given input d, and okd is network output for the kth unit given input d.

  8. Gradient Descent • The derivative of the output of a sigmoid unit given the net input is ¶oj/ ¶netj = oj(1 - oj) • This can be used to derive a learning rule which performs gradient descent in weight space in an attempt to minimize the error function. wji = -(E / wji)

  9. Backpropogation Learning Rule • Each weight wji is changed by wji = djoi dj = oj (1 - oj) (tj - oj) if j is an output unit dj = oj (1 - oj) Sdk wkj otherwise where h is a constant called the learning rate, tj is the correct output for unit j, dj is an error measure for unit j. • First determine the error for the output units, then backpropagate this error layer by layer through the network, changing weights appropriately at each layer.

  10. Backpropogation Learning Algorithm • Create a three layer network with N hidden units and fully connect input units to hidden units and hidden units to output units with small random weights. Until all examples produce the correct output within e or the mean­squared error ceases to decrease (or other termination criteria): Begin epoch For each example in training set do: Compute the network output for this example. Compute the error between this output and the correct output. Backpropagate this error and adjust weights to decrease this error. End epoch • Since continuous outputs only approach 0 or 1 in the limit, must allow for some e­approximation to learn binary functions.

  11. Comments on Training • There is no guarantee of convergence, may oscillate or reach a local minima. • However, in practice many large networks can be adequately trained on large amounts of data for realistic problems. • Many epochs (thousands) may be needed for adequate training, large data sets may require hours or days of CPU time. • Termination criteria can be: • Fixed number of epochs • Threshold on training set error

  12. Representational Power Multi­layer sigmoidal networks are very expressive. • Boolean functions: Any Boolean function can be represented by a two layer network by simulating a two­layer AND­OR network. But number of required hidden units can grow exponentially in the number of inputs. • Continuous functions: Any bounded continuous function can be approximated with arbitrarily small error by a two­layer network. Sigmoid functions provide a set of basis functions from which arbitrary functions can be composed, just as any function can be represented by a sum of sine waves in Fourier analysis. • Arbitrary functions: Any function can be approximated to arbitarary accuracy by a three­layer network.

  13. Sample Learned XOR Network 3.11 6.96 -7.38 Hidden unit A represents ¬(X Ù Y) Hidden unit B represents ¬(X Ú Y) Output O represents: A Ù ¬B ¬(X Ù Y) Ù (X Ú Y) X Å Y -2.03 -5.24 B A -3.58 -5.57 -3.6 -5.74 X Y

  14. Hidden Unit Representations • Trained hidden units can be seen as newly constructed features that re­represent the examples so that they are linearly separable. • On many real problems, hidden units can end up representing interesting recognizable features such as vowel­detectors, edge­detectors, etc. • However, particularly with many hidden units, they become more “distributed” and are hard to interpret.

  15. Input/Output Coding • Appropriate coding of inputs and outputs can make learning problem easier and improve generalization. • Best to encode each binary feature as a separate input unit and for multi­valued features include one binary unit per value rather than trying to encode input information in fewer units using binary coding or continuous values.

  16. I/O Coding cont. • Continuous inputs can be handled by a single input by scaling them between 0 and 1. • For disjoint categorization problems, best to have one output unit per category rather than encoding n categories into log n bits. Continuous output values then represent certainty in various categories. Assign test cases to the category with the highest output. • Continuous outputs (regression) can also be handled by scaling between 0 and 1.

  17. Neural Net Conclusions • Learned concepts can be represented by networks of linear threshold units and trained using gradient descent. • Analogy to the brain and numerous successful applications have generated significant interest. • Generally much slower to train than other learning methods, but exploring a rich hypothesis space that seems to work well in many domains. • Potential to model biological and cognitive phenomenon and increase our understanding of real neural systems. • Backprop itself is not very biologically plausible

  18. Natural Language Processing • What’s the goal?

  19. Communication • Communication for the speaker: • Intention: Decided why, when, and what information should be transmitted. May require planning and reasoning about agents' goals and beliefs. • Generation: Translating the information to be communicated into a string of words. • Synthesis: Output of string in desired modality, e.g.text on a screen or speech.

  20. Communication (cont.) • Communication for the hearer: • Perception: Mapping input modality to a string of words, e.g. optical character recognition or speech recognition. • Analysis: Determining the information content of the string. • Syntactic interpretation (parsing): Find correct parse tree showing the phrase structure • Semantic interpretation: Extract (literal) meaning of the string in some representation, e.g. FOPC. • Pragmatic interpretation: Consider effect of overall context on the meaning of the sentence • Incorporation: Decide whether or not to believe the content of the string and add it to the KB.

  21. Ambiguity • Natural language sentences are highly ambiguous and must be disambiguated. I saw the man on the hill with the telescope. I saw the Grand Canyon flying to LA. I saw a jet flying to LA. Time flies like an arrow. Horse flies like a sugar cube. Time runners like a coach. Time cars like a Porsche.

  22. Syntax • Syntax concerns the proper ordering of words and its effect on meaning. The dog bit the boy. The boy bit the dog. * Bit boy the dog the Colorless green ideas sleep furiously.

  23. Semantics • Semantics concerns of meaning of words, phrases, and sentences. Generally restricted to “literal meaning” • “plant” as a photosynthetic organism • “plant” as a manufacturing facility • “plant” as the act of sowing

  24. Pragmatics • Pragmatics concerns the overall commuinicative and social context and its effect on interpretation. • Can you pass the salt? • Passerby: Does your dog bite? Clouseau: No. Passerby: (pets dog) Chomp! I thought you said your dog didn't bite!! Clouseau:That, sir, is not my dog!

  25. Modular Processing Speech recognition Parsing acoustic/ phonetic syntax semantics pragmatics Sound waves words Parse trees literal meaning meaning

  26. Examples • Phonetics “grey twine” vs. “great wine” “youth in Asia” vs. “euthanasia” “yawanna” ­> “do you want to” • Syntax I ate spaghetti with a fork. I ate spaghetti with meatballs.

  27. More Examples • Semantics I put the plant in the window. Ford put the plant in Mexico. The dog is in the pen. The ink is in the pen. • Pragmatics The ham sandwich wants another beer. John thinks vanilla.

  28. Formal Grammars • A grammar is a set of production rules which generates a set of strings (a language) by rewriting the top symbol S. • Nonterminal symbols are intermediate results that are not contained in strings of the language. S ­> NP VP NP ­> Det N VP ­> V NP

  29. Terminal symbols are the final symbols (words) that compose the strings in the language. • Production rules for generating words from part of speech categories constitute the lexicon. • N ­> boy • V ­> eat

  30. Context-Free Grammars • A context­free grammar only has productions with a single symbol on the left­hand side. • CFG: S ­> NP V NP ­> Det N VP ­> V NP • not CFG: A B ­> C B C ­> F G

  31. Simplified English Grammar S ­> NP VP S ­> VP NP ­> Det Adj* N NP ­> ProN NP ­> PName VP ­> V VP ­> V NP VP ­> VP PP PP ­> Prep NP Adj* ­> e Adj* ­> Adj Adj* Lexicon: ProN ­> I; ProN ­> you; ProN ­> he; ProN ­> she Name ­> John; Name ­> Mary Adj ­> big; Adj ­> little; Adj ­> blue; Adj ­> red Det ­> the; Det ­> a; Det ­> an N ­> man; N ­> telescope; N ­> hill; N ­> saw Prep ­> with; Prep ­> for; Prep ­> of; Prep ­> in V ­> hit; V­> took; V­> saw; V ­> likes

  32. Parse Trees • A parse tree shows the derivation of a sentence in the language from the start symbol to the terminal symbols. • If a given sentence has more than one possible derivation (parse tree), it is said to be syntactically ambiguous.

  33. Syntactic Parsing • Given a string of words, determine if it is grammatical, i.e. if it can be derived from a particular grammar. • The derivation itself may also be of interest. • Normally want to determine all possible parse trees and then use semantics and pragmatics to eliminate spurious parses and build a semantic representation.

  34. Parsing Complexity • Problem: Many sentences have many parses. • An English sentence with n prepositional phrases at the end has at least 2n parses. I saw the man on the hill with a telescope on Tuesday in Austin... • The actual number of parses is given by the Catalan numbers: 1, 2, 5, 14, 42, 132, 429, 1430, 4862, 16796...

  35. Parsing Algorithms • Top Down: Search the space of possible derivations of S (e.g.depth­first) for one that matches the input sentence. I saw the man. S ­> NP VP NP ­> Det Adj* N Det ­> the Det ­> a Det ­> an NP ­> ProN ProN ­> I VP ­> V NP V ­> hit V ­> took V ­> saw NP ­> Det Adj* N Det ­> the Adj* ­> e N ­> man

  36. Parsing Algorithms (cont.) • Bottom Up: Search upward from words finding larger and larger phrases until a sentence is found. I saw the man. ProN saw the man ProN ­> I NP saw the man NP ­> ProN NP N the man N ­> saw (dead end) NP V the man V ­> saw NP V Det man Det ­> the NP V Det Adj* man Adj* ­> e NP V Det Adj* N N ­> man NP V NP NP ­> Det Adj* N NP VP VP ­> V NP S S ­> NP VP

  37. Bottom­up Parsing Algorithm function BOTTOM­UP­PARSE(words, grammar) returns a parse tree forestwords loop do if LENGTH(forest) = 1 and CATEGORY(forest[1]) = START(grammar) then returnforest[1] else ichoose from {1...LENGTH(forest)} rulechoose from RULES(grammar) n LENGTH(RULE­RHS(rule)) subsequence SUBSEQUENCE(forest, i, i+n­1) if MATCH(subsequence, RULE­RHS(rule)) then forest[i...i+n­1] / [MAKE­NODE(RULE­LHS(rule), subsequence)] elsefail end

More Related