1 / 35

Norway Keynote 2010

Norway Keynote 2010. Professor Ray Bull University of Leicester. 2 Principles of Investigative Interviewing.

alban
Download Presentation

Norway Keynote 2010

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Norway Keynote 2010 Professor Ray Bull University of Leicester

  2. 2 Principles of Investigative Interviewing Many of you will be aware that in England and Wales in 1992 the ‘Steering Group On Investigative Interviewing’ (set up by the Association of Chief Police Officers and the Home Office – which was the relevant government ministry) approved for the first time a number of “Principles of Investigative Interviewing” which included • “The role of investigative interviewing is to obtain accurate and reliable information from suspects, witnesses or victims in order to discover the truth…” • “Investigative interviewing should be approached with an open mind” • “Information obtained from the person being interviewed should always tested against what the interviewing officer already knows…” • “When questioning anyone a police officer must act fairly…” • “Vulnerable people, whether victims, witnesses or suspects, must be treated with particular consideration at all times”.

  3. 3. New booklets and training courses (For more on the early evolution of the notion of ‘investigative interviewing’ see Williamson, 1993; the ‘Preface’ in Bull, Valentine, and Williamson, 2009.) This “Steering Group” largely involved experienced police officers who took advice from a variety of sources, including the relevant psychological literature and psychologists. Also in 1992 • two new booklets on the new PEACE approach to investigative interviewing (which adheres to the principles I have just mentioned) were distributed to every police officer in England and Wales (all 127,000 of them) • the new PEACE training courses began. (For accounts of the PEACE approach see Bull and Milne, 2004; Milne and Bull, 1999.)

  4. 4. Advice from professionals in the USA In 2002 Holmes (formerly of the Miami Police Department) published a book entitled Criminal interrogation: A modern format for interrogating suspectsin which he stated (p.x) that “When you finish reading this book, I hope you have one predominate thought, ‘You don’t obtain confessions by asking the suspect questions. You have to convince a suspect to confess by the use of persuasive interrogational arguments’”. With regard to questioning, Holmes stated that “The literature is devoid of information on how to question. Oddly enough, I don’t even know of a law school that teaches the act of cross-examination” (p.15). Whereas this may be an accurate account of Holmes’ knowledge, it is not a valid account of the literature on questioning available world-wide. Holmes continued that “The primary purpose of questioning is to determine that the transition to accusatory interrogation should be made” (p.16).

  5. 5 Once a suspect has confessed Holmes (2002) recommended that a “formal confession should be taken without delay while a suspect is in a cooperative mood” (p.115). He also recommended that this post-interrogation/interview formal confession be audio or video recorded, even though he noted that “There is, however, one major drawback ….. In court, the investigator is invariably asked ….. ‘Why didn’t you record the entire interview of the defendant?’ ”. He stated that “I never felt entirely comfortable answering that question. I never said it, but I often wanted to say ‘I didn’t want to give you a sword you could stick in me’ ” (p.116).

  6. 6. More advice from the USA Fairly recently in 2005 in the FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin an instructor (Boetig) in the Law Enforcement Communication Unit at the FBI contended that “…interrogations are less of a conversation than a monologue by investigators in which they provide suspects with acceptable reasons to confess” (p.13/14). These reasons were said to be of one of three types – rationalisation, projection of blame, and minimisation, the first of which “offers suspects the opportunity to make the crimes appear soundly acceptable”; in relation to this Boetig stated that “Investigators may rationalise a crime merely by explaining to the suspect that the deviant act was logical behaviour that anyone in his position would have done …” (p.15). the second “distances suspects from appearing solely responsible”; an example of projection could involve putting “… blame on the people that taught them to be criminals, such as siblings, peers, parents, and fellow inmates” (p.18). and the third “produces less guilt or shame for the suspect” (p.14); Boetig suggested that an example of minimisation could involve “… an explanation of his genetic predispositions to deviance….” (p.16).

  7. 7. But has such advice been based on research? In 2010 Chris Meissner and colleagues noted that in the USA “ …interrogative methods currently available to law enforcement have no scientific foundation but rather have been offered (or better, sold) by former investigators who purport that the validity of those techniques is based on their success…” (Meissner, Russano, & Narchet, 2010, p. 122-123).

  8. 8. The views of suspects? Neither of the books that I have so far mentioned seemed to have benefitted from research on the views of suspects/offenders concerning how they were interviewed. Holmberg and Christianson’s (2002) ground-breaking research study (also see Deslauriers-Varin & St-Yves,2009; Kebbell, Alison & Hurren, 2008; St-Yves & Deslauriers-Varin, 2006) emphasised that suspects/offenders interviewed in a dominant, coercive way may be lees likely to confess than those interviewed in a humane way. In 2005 O’Connor and Carson (both highly experienced professional interviewers) found that the predominant reason the confessors (to child molestation) gave for why they confessed was the respect shown to them by the interviewers.

  9. 9. Resistance Nearly twenty five years ago Gudjonsson and Clark’s new (1986) theory focussed on three main factors that are likely to influence suspects’ coping in police interviews. The resultant coping strategy would affect whether suspects became suggestible or resistant. If interviewers provided ‘negative feedback’ some suspects will respond to this with ‘resistance’. Such resistance may also arise in suspects who are/become suspicious of the interviewers’ intentions. (Also see Gudjonsson, 2006.)

  10. 10. Research in the USA One of the most important studies ever published concerning what actually happens when the police interview suspects is by Leo (1996). This seminal study, conducted in the USA, is deserving of wider and deeper attention around the world than it has heretofore received. Leo noted that the Court (Justice Earl Warren) in Miranda V Arizona (1966) commented on the absence of first-hand knowledge of what actually takes place during police interviews with suspects and that it called for research on this topic. In the ensuing thirty years little such research was conducted.

  11. 11. Leo (cont’d) Leo’s own study was based on his nine months of ‘fieldwork’ with the Criminal Investigation Division of a large, urban police department (thought by many, Leo reports, to be one of the leading police forces in the USA). He sat in on 122 interviews (involving 45 detectives) and he also watched video recordings of a further 60 interviews conducted by two other police organisations. In his 1996 publication Leo demonstrated good awareness of the possible influence his presence had on the 122 live interviews and he was able to compare these with the 60 recorded interviews (where he, of course, was not present). He noted that he tended to be excluded from the more serious cases, though this was not always so. Leo coded each of the interviews for 25 potential tactics/techniques which he derived from those (i) advocated in police manuals, (ii) taught on training courses, and (iii) used in popular culture.

  12. 12. Leo (cont’d) Leo found that the detectives usually began by confronting the suspects with evidence suggesting guilt, either real/true evidence (85% of interrogations) or false evidence(30%); then the detectives attempted (in 43%) to undermine the suspects’ denials. Leo also noted that the detectives • often appealed to the suspects’ self-interest in confessing (88%); • mentioned the importance of the suspect co-operating (37%); • offered moral justifications or psychological excuses (34%); • used praise/flattery (30%); • mentioned the detectives’ expertise (29%); • minimised the seriousness of the offence (22%); • appealed to the suspects’ conscience (22%).

  13. 13. Leo (cont’d) Leo defined an interrogation as ‘successful’ if the suspect provided during it some incriminating information. (Around two thirds met this definition.) The only factors that he found to be associated with whether an interrogation involved some such success were (i) its length and (ii) the number of tactics used. Significant associations between some of the tactics and the suspect providing incriminating information were found as follows: • identifying contradictions (91% of the interrogations in which this tactic was used); [this tactic/skill is now being emphasised in the PEACE approach] • offering the suspect a moral justification or psychological excuse (91%) • praise/flattery (91%) • appealing to suspect’s conscience (97%). Other outstanding studies of actual police interviews in North America include Feld (2006) and King and Snook (2009).

  14. 14 (of 28). Views of interviewers More recent information concerning USA police use of techniques while interviewing suspects comes from a survey of interviewing/interrogating officers’ (Reppucci, Meyer, & Kostelnik, 2010). The percentages of respondents who endorsed various techniques included • observe body language to detect deception - 92% • observe speech patterns to detect deception - 57% • suggest what may have happened – 46% • discourage denials – 29% • false evidence - 27%. (But see Kebbell, Hurren, and Roberts (2006) who found that telling ‘suspects’ about evidence against them that was incorrect reduced their likelihood of confessing; also see van Bergen, Jelicic, and Merckelbach, 2008.) For more surveys of investigating officers’ views see (for North America) Kassin, et al. (2007); (for Finland) Hakkanen, Ask, Kebbell, Alison, and Granhag (2009); (for Australia) Alison and Howard (2005); (for England ) Soukara, Bull, and Vrij (2002); (for India) Alison, Sarangi, and Wright (2008); (for Hong Kong) Alison, Kebbell, and Leung (2008).

  15. 15. A human approach recommended in the USA An experienced investigator in the USA who ‘foresaw’ (but did not mention) the possibility that a PEACE-like approach may be useful in the USA was Yeschke. In the second edition of his The art of investigative interviewing (2003) this former FBI special agent with more than 30 years of relevant experience presented what the second part of his book’s title refers to as A human approach to testimonial evidence. Yeschke stated that “Criminal investigations must be conducted in a professional atmosphere in which no one tampers with evidence and no one physically or psychologically compels an innocent person to confess” (p.7 - but no mention here of unduly compelling a guilty person to confess).

  16. 16. Yeschke (cont’d) Yeschkenoted that “The tactics suggested in this book ….are ethical” (p.12) and that “I consider the following behaviours to be unethical: • Using interrogation tactics instead of interviewing tactics • Treating each interviewee as though culpable … • Using coercion … • Not maintaining the interviewee’s dignity” (p.12). Yeschke stated that “These and similar tactics have been used in the past … It is time for a change “ (p.12). He emphasised the importance of “actively listening” (p.21), that “An interview is more of a marathon than a sprint” (p.22), that “The more effective you are in collecting testimonial evidence in an interview, the more proficient you will be as an investigator” (p.36), and that “… interviews necessarily take place to uncover the truth …” (p.41).

  17. 17. Some of our studies In a 2010 book chapter we concluded that our “Study 1 found that the views of experienced interviewers/detectives in England appeared to be in line with the new approach (referred to as investigative interviewing) and its evolving training program (called PEACE). One major aim of Studies 2 and 3 was to determine if actual police interviewing of suspects was in line with the philosophy behind investigative interviewing. These two studies found this to be the case to a considerable extent (e.g., the absence of oppressive coercive tactics), which clearly suggests that the PEACE approach can be accepted and employed by police officers.” (Bull & Soukara, 2010).

  18. 18. Our studies (cont’d) However, our studies also found that some inappropriate tactics were used in a considerable number of interviews (such as leading questions, repeating the same questions, positive confrontation), though Clarke, Milne, and Bull (in revision) found fewer leading questions used in police interviews with suspects.

  19. 19 (of 28). Our studies (cont’d) Our studies 3 and 4 also had the major aim of trying to determine if interviewer use of tactics/skills bore any relationship to the suspect confessing. Contrary to what the limited previous research had found (for example, Baldwin, 1993), most of the suspects who confessed did not do so very early on. This allowed, almost for the first time, examination of whether how the interviewer behaves does influence confessing (as suggested by convicted persons in studies by Holmberg & Christiansson, 2002; Kebbel & Hurren, 2006). We found that the interviewers employed non-coercive techniques and that the PEACE/investigative interviewing approach nevertheless resulted in confessions.

  20. 20 (of 28). Police-researcher relationships We concluded that “Overall, this chapter demonstrates that when a sufficiently good relationship can be established (taking many years), enlightened police officers/police organizations are sometimes willing to allow comprehensive scrutiny of their interviewing performance by psychologists. Perhaps they had the confidence to do this because a national policy plus a comprehensive training program (i.e. PEACE) had been in place for several years and they therefore thought that the interviews in their fairly large police force would be, at least, of reasonable standard. If so, they were correct.” Nevertheless, it is important to note what the American psychologist DeClue said in his extensive commentary on police interviews with suspects - “ … law enforcement officers are likely to see psychologists as being the enemy when the law enforcement officers have not done their jobs well” (2005, p. 351).

  21. 21 (of 28). Can the PEACE approach be recommended In concluding our 2010 chapter we stated that “The tactics of greatest concern to some eminent psychologists (e.g., Gudjonsson, 2003) were very rarely present, though some that are likely to be unreliable (e.g., leading questions) were often used—perhaps due to their probable high frequency in everyday life. Thus, the new research presented in this chapter could be taken to suggest that police organisations around the world should actively consider adopting the PEACE approach and associated training programs.”

  22. 22. “Yes we can” (Obama) In the “Conclusion” chapter of their 2010 edited volume Chris Meissner and Dan Lassiter said that “One alternative…” to “…psychologically coercive interrogative methods…” is “…the PEACE approach to investigative interviewing…used in Great Britain…” (Meissner & Lassiter, 2010, p. 229). In the “Afterword” chapter Saul Kassin stated that “Although more systematic research is needed, it is clear that investigative interviewing offers a potentially effective alternative to the classic American interrogation” (p. 235).

  23. 23 (of 28). Research involving experiments In 2010 Chris Meissner and colleagues reminded us that “The interrogation room presents a challenge to laboratory researchers who attempt to recreate the elements of police interrogation in a controlled environment.” (Meissner, et al., 2010, p. 118). From their recent laboratory based research in which “The inquisitorial approach used was modeled after the approach advocated in Great Britain”, they concluded that “ The results of both studies demonstrated a significant advantage in using inquisitorial methods over that of accusatorial approaches” (p. 122). (Also see Vrij, Mann, and Fisher, 2006.)

  24. 24 (of 28). PEACEin Canada? A few months ago in Canada Snook, Eastwood, Stinson, Tedeschini, and House (2010) - in a journal article co-authored by three police officers and two academics - stated that “…interviewing practices in Canada require substantive reform” (p. 215). They recommended adoption of • the PEACE approach commenting that it “…is ethical and grounded in scientific research…” (p. 215) • “…a standardized tier-based training program for interviewing…” (p. 224). [For accounts of such a program see Bull and Milne (2004); Milne, Shaw, and Bull (2007).]

  25. 25 (of 28) Snook et al. in describing the PEACE approach said that “By obtaining an initial free narrative and using evidence-based challenges – that is, an interviewee’s failure to respond truthfully to evidence (e.g., DNA, CCTV recordings) that contradicts his or her account – can illustrate guilt regardless of whether the interviewee verbalizes that guilt. PEACE therefore, allows the truth to be revealed without the use of manipulative and coercive tactics and the risk of false confessions” (p. 223). [Hershkowitz, Horowitz, Lamb, Orbach, and Sternberg (2004) found an association between (youhful) suspects admitting to child abuse in interviews and their responsiveness to free recall prompts.]

  26. 26 (of 28). PEACEin our time? Some countries have already decided to adopt the PEACE approach. These include New Zealand, which commissioned a world-wide review of the relevant literature and current police practices (Schollum,2005). In Norway in 2002 a national investigative interviewing training course was commissioned. This ‘KREATIV’ approach/model was “heavily influenced by the British PEACE course” (Fahsing & Rachlew, 2009, p. 42) and the 2006 feedback from officers who were trained in this model was very positive.

  27. 27 of 28. How/when to present evidence In the conclusion of his in-depth, overview chapter Gudjonsson (2006) stated that “Usually, suspects confess for a combination of reasons, but perception of the strength of evidence is the single most important reason. This has important implications for investigators” (p. 143). (Also see Moston, Stephenson, and Williamson, 1992.) The various ways that the ‘evidence’ can be presented to suspects in investigative interviews (i.e. that follow the PEACE approach) is now being studied experimentally (i.e. using good ‘scientific’ methodology).

  28. 28 (of 28). The ‘GRIMACE’ approach Later in this conference my colleague Dr. Coral Dando will be presenting a paper concerned with our ongoing research on this very point. (Also see Sellers and Kebbell, 2009; van der Sleen, 2009.) Our research examines the effectiveness of the ‘GRIMACE’ approach which involves Gathering Reliable Information, then Motivating an Account, then Challenging this Effectively.

  29. References Alison, L. & Howard, J. (2005). The interpersonal dynamics of police interviewing. In L. Alison (Ed.) The forensic psychologist’s casebook. (pp. 114 – 141). Cullompton: Willan. Alison, L., Kebbell, M., & Leung, J. (2008). A facet analysis of police officers’ self-reported use of suspect interviewing strategies and their discomfort with ambiguity. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1072-1087. Alison, L., Sarangi, S., & Wright, A. (2008). Human rights is not enough: the need for demonstrating efficacy of an ethical approach to interviewing in India. Legal & Criminological Psychology, 13, 89-106. Boetig, B. (2005). Reducing a guilty suspect’s resistance to confessing: Applying criminological theory to interrogation theme development. FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin, August, 13-20. Bull,R., & Soukara, S. (2010). Four studies of what really happens in police interviews. In G. D. Lassiter and C. Meissner (Eds.) Police interrogations and false confessions: Current research, practice, and policy recommendations (pp. 81-96). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

  30. Bull, R., Valentine, T., & Williamson, T. (2009). Handbook of psychology of investigative interviewing. Chichester: Wiley. DeClue, G. (2005). Psychological consultation in cases involving interrogations and confessions. Journal of Psychiatry and Law, 33, 313-358. Des Lauriers-Varin, N. and St-Yves, M. (2006). An empirical investigation of offenders’ decision to confess their crime during police interrogation. Paper presented at the Second International Investigative Interviewing Conference, Portsmouth, July. Fahsing. I., & Rachlew, A. (2009). Investigative interviewing in the Nordic region. In T. Williamson., B. Milne, & S. Savage (Eds.) International developments in investigative interviewing (pp. 39-64). Cullompton: Willan. Feld, B. (2006). Police interrogation of juveniles: An empirical study of policy and practice. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 97, 219-316. Gudjonnson, G. (2006). Sex offenders and confessions: How to overcome their resistance during questioning. Journal of Clinical Forensic Medicine, 13, 203-207.

  31. Hershkowitz, I., Horowitz, D., Lamb, M., & Sternberg, K. (2004). Interviewing youthful suspects in alleged sex crimes: A descriptive analysis. Child Abuse and Neglect, 28, 423-438. Holmberg, U., & Christianson, S-A. (2002). Murderers’ and sexual offenders’ experiences of police interviews and their inclination to admit or deny crimes. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 20, 31-45.  Holmes, W. (2002). Criminal interrogation: A modern format for interrogating criminal suspects based on the intellectual approach. Springfield, Ill.: C.C. Thomas.  Kassin, S. (2010). Deconstructing confessions – the state of the literature. In G. D. Lassiter and C. Meissner (Eds.) Police interrogations and false confessions: Current research, practice, and policy recommendations (pp. 231-238). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Kassin, S., Leo, R., Meissner, C., Richman, K., Colwell, L., Leach, A-Y., & La Fon, F. (2007). Police interviewing and interrogation: A self-report survey of police practices and beliefs. Law and Human Behavior (in press). ???????

  32. Kebbell, M., Alison, L., & Hurren, E. (2008). Sex offenders’ perceptions of the effectiveness and fairness of humanity, dominance, and displaying an understanding of cognitive distortions in police interviews: A vignette study. Psychology, Crime and Law, 14, 435-449. Kebbell, M., Hurren, E., & Roberts, S. (2006). Mock-suspects’ decisions to confess: The accuracy of eyewitness evidence is critical. Apllied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 477-486. King, L., & Snook, B. (2009). Peering inside a Canadian interrogation room: An examination of the Reid model of interrogation, influence tactics, and coercive strategies. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36, 674-694. Leo, R. (1996). Inside the interrogation room. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 86, 266 – 303. Meissner, C., & Lassiter, G. D. (2010). What have we learned? Implications for practice. In G. D. Lassiter and C. Meissner (Eds.) Police interrogations and false confessions: Current research, practice, and policy recommendations (pp. 225-230). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

  33. Meissner, C., Russano, M., & Narchet, F. (2010). The importance of laboratory science for improving the diagnostic value of confession evidence. In G. D. Lassiter and C. Meissner (Eds.) Police interrogations and false confessions: Current research, practice, and policy recommendations (pp. 111-126). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Milne, R., Shaw, G., & Bull, R. (2007). Investigative interviewing: The role of research. In D. Carson, B. Milne., F. Pakes., K. Shalev & A. Shawyer. (Eds.) Applying psychology to criminal justice. (pp. 65-80). Chichester:Wiley.  Moston, S., Stephenson, G., and Williamson, T. (1992). The effects of case characteristics on suspect behaviour during police questioning. British Journal of Criminology, 32, 23 – 40. O’Connor, T., & Carson, W. (2005). Understanding the psychology of child molesters: A key to getting confessions. The Police Chief, 72, December, 1-7.

  34. Schollum, M. (2005). Investigative interviewing – the literature. Wellington, NZ: Police National Headquarters. Sellers, S., & Kebbell, M. (2009). When should evidence be disclosed in an interview with a suspect? An experiment with mock-suspects. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling, 6, ??? -??? Snook, B., Eastwood, J., Stinson, M., Tedescheni, J., & House, J. (2010). Reforming investigative interviewing in Canada. Canadian Journal of Criminology and Criminal Justice, 52, 215-229. St-Yves, M., & Deslauriers-Varin, N. (2009). The psychology of suspects’ decision-making during interrogation. In R. Bull, T. Valentine, and T. Williamson. (Eds.) Handbook of psychology of investigative interviewing. (pp. 1-16). Chichester: Wiley.

  35. van Bergen, S., Jelicic, M., & Merckelbach. H. (2008). Interrogation techniques and memory distrust. Psychology, Crime and Law, 14, 425-434. van der Sleen, J. (2009). A structured model for investigative interviewing of suspects. In R. Bull, T. Valentine, and T. Williamson. (Eds.) Handbook of psychology of investigative interviewing. (pp. 35-52). Chichester: Wiley. Vrij, A., Mann, S., & Fisher, R. (2006). Information-gathering vs accusatory interview style: Individual differences in respondents’ experiences. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 589-599. Williamson, T. (1993). From interrogation to investigative interviewing: Strategic problems in police questioning. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 3, 89-99. Yeschke, C. (2003). The art of investigative interviewing: A human approach to testimonial evidence (second edition). New York: Butterworth Heinemann.

More Related