Sexual selection
Download
1 / 67

EVOLSXSL - PowerPoint PPT Presentation


  • 408 Views
  • Uploaded on

Sexual Selection. Selection for traits which are solely concerned with increasing mating success is usually referred to as sexual selection. Can Work in Two Ways:.

loader
I am the owner, or an agent authorized to act on behalf of the owner, of the copyrighted work described.
capcha
Download Presentation

PowerPoint Slideshow about 'EVOLSXSL' - Jims


An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation

Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author.While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server.


- - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - E N D - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -
Presentation Transcript
Sexual selection l.jpg

Sexual Selection

Selection for traits which are solely concerned with increasing mating success is usually referred to as sexual selection


Can work in two ways l.jpg
Can Work in Two Ways:

1. By favoring the ability of one sex (usually males) to compete directly with one another for fertilizations, for example by fighting – intra-sexual selection

2. By favoring traits in one sex which attracts the other sex – inter-sexual selection


Slide3 l.jpg
The intensity of sexual selection depends on the degree of competition for mates & this in turn depends on two factors

1. The difference in parental effort between the sexes

2. The ratio of males to females available for mating at any one time (known as the operational sex ratio)


Strength of sexual selection l.jpg
Strength of Sexual Selection competition for mates & this in turn depends on two factors

When parental effort is more or less equal, as in monogamous birds, for example, where both male and female feed the young, sexual selection is less intense than in species with very different levels of parental effort

If equal numbers of the two sexes come into breeding condition at the same time, the degree of sexual selection is reduced because there is less chance for a few males to control access to very large numbers of females


Strength of sexual selection5 l.jpg
Strength of Sexual Selection competition for mates & this in turn depends on two factors

In contrast, when females come into breeding condition asynchronously there’s a chance for a small number of males to control many females one after the other

With such high potential payoffs, sexual competition should be intense


Summary l.jpg
Summary competition for mates & this in turn depends on two factors

P.E.

M.E.

P.E.

M.E.

M.E.

M.E.

P.E.

P.E.

Mating

Sexual Selection

Promiscuous/Polygamous

Monogamous

Very Strong Less Strong


Slide7 l.jpg
If sexual selection is to explain the differences between the sexes, it will have to act on the sexes differently

  • A.J. Bateman (1948) predicted that sexual selection – variation in mating success – will usually be a more potent force in the evolution of males than in the evolution of females

  • In other words, access to mates will be a limiting resource for males but not females

  • Of course this prediction is central to the theory of sexual selection


Direct tests l.jpg

Direct Tests the sexes, it will have to act on the sexes differently

Jones, A.G., J.R. Arguello, and S.J. Arnold. 2002. Validation of Bateman’s principles: a genetic study of sexual selection and mating patterns in the rough-skinned newt. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 269:2533-2539.

Jones, A.G., G. Rosenqvist, A. Berglund, et al. 2000. The Bateman gradient and the cause of sexual selection in a sex-role-reversed pipefish. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 267:677-680.


Rough skinned newt l.jpg
Rough-Skinned Newt the sexes, it will have to act on the sexes differently


Broad nosed pipefish l.jpg
Broad-nosed Pipefish the sexes, it will have to act on the sexes differently


Sexual selection theory l.jpg
Sexual Selection Theory the sexes, it will have to act on the sexes differently

  • Jones et al.’s study on pipefish shows that greater sexual selection on males than on females is not inherent in the identity of the sexes themselves

  • When access is limiting for females instead of males we predict that females will compete with each other over access to males and males will be choosy


Competition for mates l.jpg
Competition for Mates the sexes, it will have to act on the sexes differently

The most dramatic and obvious way in which males compete for mates is by fighting and ritualized contests, and males often have evolved weapons for fighting

Males may dispute over direct access to females or over places where females are likely to go


Male male competition intrasexual selection l.jpg

Male-Male Competition: Intrasexual Selection the sexes, it will have to act on the sexes differently

Wikelski, M. and F. Trillmich. 1997. Body size and sexual size dimorphism in marine iguanas fluctuate as result of opposing natural and sexual selection: an island comparison. Evolution 51:922-936


Although intense fighting over females can occur males often compete in less conspicuous ways l.jpg

Although intense fighting over females can occur, males often compete in less conspicuous ways

Abele, L.G. and S. Gilchrist. 1977. Homosexual rape and sexual selection in acanthocephalan worms. Science


Abele and gilchrist 1977 l.jpg
Abele and Gilchrist (1977) often compete in less conspicuous ways

The males of Moniliformes dubius, a parasitic acanthocephalan worm found in the intestine of rats cements up the females genital opening after copulation to prevent other males from fertilizing her

In addition to sealing up the female after copulation, the male sometimes “copulates” with rival males and applies cement to their genital region to prevent them from mating again


Carayon 1974 l.jpg
Carayon (1974) often compete in less conspicuous ways

Shows that the male hemipteran Xylocoris maculipennis pierces the body wall of the female and injects sperm during copulation fertilizing her eggs

Like with acanthocephalan worms, males sometimes engage in homosexual “copulation”. A male injects his sperm into a rival male where they wait to be passed on to a female next time the victim mates


Female choice l.jpg
Female Choice often compete in less conspicuous ways

Since females in the majority of species invest more than males in each offspring, they might be expected to be choosier in selecting their mates

Females often select males on the basis of material resources that they can offer or females may select males on the basis of genetic benefits for their offspring


Female choice21 l.jpg
Female Choice often compete in less conspicuous ways

Non-genetic benefits or material resources might include male defended breeding territories containing resources that play a crucial role in the survival of a female’s eggs or young

Females may also choose whether or not to mate with a male on the basis of his ability to provide food. In some birds and insects, for example, males may provide food for the female during courtship making a significant contribution to her eggs


Slide22 l.jpg

For example, female hanging flies, often compete in less conspicuous waysHylobittacus apicalis, will mate with a male only if he provides a large insect for her to eat during copulation

The larger the insect the longer the male is allowed to copulate and the more eggs he fertilizes. The female gains from a large insect by having more food to put into her eggs


Genetic benefits l.jpg
Genetic Benefits often compete in less conspicuous ways

If some males have better genes than others, could a female improve the success of her progeny by choosing males with good genes?

Good genes are the ones which increase the ability of her offspring to survive, compete and reproduce


Good genes l.jpg

Good Genes often compete in less conspicuous ways

Welch, A., R.D. Semlitsch and H. C. Gerhardt. 1998. Call duration as an indicator of genetic quality in male gray tree frogs. Science 280:1928-1930.


Fitness of long calling frogs vs short calling frogs l.jpg
Fitness of long-calling frogs vs. short-calling frogs often compete in less conspicuous ways

1995 1996

Fitness Measure HF LF HF LF

Larval Growth NS LCB LCB LCB

Time to Metamorphosis LCB NS LCB NS

Mass at Metamorphosis NS LCB NS NS

Larval Survival LCB NS NS NS

Postmetamorphic growth - - NS LCB

HF – High food

LF – Low food

NS – non-significant

LCB – Long call better


Elaborate displays l.jpg
Elaborate Displays often compete in less conspicuous ways

The theory of sexual selection is most famous as an attempt to explain the evolution of exclusively elaborate adornments and displays

Although some elaborate displays may have evolved for use in contests between males, some have certainly evolved as a result of selection by females for genetic benefits


Two hypotheses to explain how selection for genetic benefits might produce elaborate traits l.jpg
Two Hypotheses to Explain how Selection for Genetic Benefits might Produce Elaborate Traits

1. Fisher’s hypothesis (also called the runaway process)

2. The Handicap hypothesis


Slide30 l.jpg

Many studies have shown a relationship between male mating success or female preference and male sexual displays

Andersson, M. 1982. Female choice selects for extreme tail length in a widowbird. Nature


Andersson 1982 l.jpg
Andersson (1982) success or female preference and male sexual displays

Showed that females of the long tailed widow bird in Kenya prefer males with long tails

This is a highly polygynous species making it an ideal candidate for sexual selection; the male is a sparrow sized bird with a tail up to 50cm long

The female’s tail is about 7cm long, presumably close to the optimum for flight purposes


Andersson 198232 l.jpg
Andersson (1982) success or female preference and male sexual displays

Studied 36 males which he divided into 4 groups

One group he docked the tails to about 14cm

Another group he increased tail lengths by 25cm, gluing on cut feathers

The remaining two groups served as controls - one left untouched & the other has their tails cut and glued without altering length


Conclusion l.jpg
Conclusion success or female preference and male sexual displays

Male ornaments are favored by female mate choice and probably evolved through it

Not due to intra-sexual selection among males competing for territories or hierarchy ranks - all males held territories equally


Hypotheses l.jpg
Hypotheses success or female preference and male sexual displays

Fishers Hypothesis (1930) - postulates runaway feedback between female preference and male displays

Imagine at the start there was a range of tail lengths and female preferences in the population

Females with a preference for slightly longer than average tails would be mated to males with longer tails

The crucial fact to note is that offspring of these mating would have both the tail and preference genes


Hypotheses36 l.jpg
Hypotheses success or female preference and male sexual displays

Fishers Hypothesis

  • The preference is expressed only in females and the tail in males, but everyone carries both kinds of genes

  • In short, there will arise an association or covariance between tail and preference genes


Hypotheses37 l.jpg
Hypotheses success or female preference and male sexual displays

The Handicap Hypothesis - Zahavi (1975) suggested an alternative view of elaborate male sexual displays

  • He suggested that females prefer long tails (or other equivalent traits) because they are handicaps and therefore act as reliable signals of a males genetic quality

  • The tail demonstrates a males ability to survive in spite of the handicap, which means that he must be extra good in other respects


Hypotheses38 l.jpg
Hypotheses success or female preference and male sexual displays

Zahavi’s Hypothesis

  • If any of this ability is heritable, then the tendency to be good at surviving will be passed on to the offspring

  • Therefore females select for good genes by selecting to mate only with males whose displays honestly indicate their genetic quality


Slide39 l.jpg

It is important to note that in this hypothesis the good genes are genes for survival and reproduction, rather than genes purely for attracting females, as assumed in Fisher’s hypothesis


Evidence for the fisher and handicap hypotheses l.jpg
Evidence for the Fisher and Handicap Hypotheses genes are genes for survival and reproduction, rather than genes purely for attracting females, as assumed in Fisher’s hypothesis

To demonstrate that a trait had evolved by Fisher’s process, it would be necessary to show that there is genetic variation for both female preference and the male trait and that genes tend to covary

Because Fisher’s hypothesis assumes that the only benefit of the selected trait is increased mating success, it would be necessary to show that expression of the male trait did not correlate with any inherited aspect of fitness


Support for fisher s hypothesis l.jpg

Support for Fisher’s Hypothesis genes are genes for survival and reproduction, rather than genes purely for attracting females, as assumed in Fisher’s hypothesis

Houde, A.E. and J.A. Endler. 1990. Correlated evolution of female mating preferences and male color patterns in the guppy Poecilia reticulata. Science


Houde and endler 1990 l.jpg
Houde and Endler (1990) genes are genes for survival and reproduction, rather than genes purely for attracting females, as assumed in Fisher’s hypothesis

Males from different populations differ greatly in the extent to which they develop bright orange and blue spots, which are a stimulus for females during courtship

Females from streams with large spotted males have stronger preferences for males with large orange spots than streams with small-spotted males


Houde and endler 199043 l.jpg
Houde and Endler (1990) genes are genes for survival and reproduction, rather than genes purely for attracting females, as assumed in Fisher’s hypothesis

The difference between populations in both male sexual color pattern and female preference are genetic: they persist in the lab for many generations

The fact that the differences persist under lab conditions suggests that the expression does not depend on the ability to gather food or on disease resistance - i.e., results are consistent with Fisher’s hypothesis


Handicap hypothesis l.jpg

Handicap Hypothesis genes are genes for survival and reproduction, rather than genes purely for attracting females, as assumed in Fisher’s hypothesis

Mainly focused on

Hamilton, W.D. and M. Zuk. 1982. Heritable true fitness and bright birds: a role for parasites? Science

- namely that sexual displays are reliable indictors of genetic resistance to disease


One of the most detailed studies to date l.jpg

One of the most detailed studies to date genes are genes for survival and reproduction, rather than genes purely for attracting females, as assumed in Fisher’s hypothesis

Moller, A.P. 1988. Female choice selects for male sexual tail ornaments in the monogamous swallow. Nature

Moller, A.P. 1990. Effects of a haematophagous mite on the barn swallow: a test of the Hamilton and Zuk hypothesis. Evolution


Moller 1988 l.jpg
Moller (1988) genes are genes for survival and reproduction, rather than genes purely for attracting females, as assumed in Fisher’s hypothesis

Moller demostrates that females prefer males with longer tails

Males with experimentally elongated tails paired up more quickly and were also preferred by females seeking extra-pair matings


Why do females prefer males with longer tails l.jpg
Why do females prefer males with longer tails? genes are genes for survival and reproduction, rather than genes purely for attracting females, as assumed in Fisher’s hypothesis

Moller (1990) - Could the ornament signal a males genetic quality in terms of his ability to resist parasites?

Swallows carry a blood-sucking mite, Ornithonyssus bursa, which infects both adults and nestlings


Moller 1990 l.jpg
Moller (1990) genes are genes for survival and reproduction, rather than genes purely for attracting females, as assumed in Fisher’s hypothesis

The life cycle of the mite, from egg to adult, lasts just 5-7 days so one reproductive cycle of the swallow provides time for 8-10 generations of mites (up to 14,000 mites)

Moller showed that nestlings reared in nests with lots of mites were lighter and smaller and suffered increased mortality. Removal experiments confirmed that mites were the cause of the reduced growth


Moller 199054 l.jpg
Moller (1990) genes are genes for survival and reproduction, rather than genes purely for attracting females, as assumed in Fisher’s hypothesis

There was large variation in the population in the degree of parasite infection

To test whether parasite resistance was heritable, Moller exchanged half the nestlings between pairs of nests


Slide55 l.jpg

Nestling parasite burden was correlated with that of its parents, even when it was reared in another’s nest - suggests that resistance is at least partly genetic

# of mites

on own

offsping

in other

nest

# of mites on

male parent


Link with the swallow s tail l.jpg

Link with the swallow’s tail parents, even when it was reared in another’s nest - suggests that resistance is at least partly genetic

# mites

on own

offspring

in other

nest

Moller found that parents with longer tails had offspring with smaller mite loads, even when their offspring were raised in another nest

males tail length (mm)


Moller 199058 l.jpg
Moller (1990) parents, even when it was reared in another’s nest - suggests that resistance is at least partly genetic

This suggests that the length of a male’s tail signals his degree of parasite resistance

This fits the Hamilton-Zuk/Handicap hypothesis that females are choosing males able to pass on good genes to their offspring


Slide59 l.jpg

Female Choice & Sexual Selection parents, even when it was reared in another’s nest - suggests that resistance is at least partly genetic

Seehausen, O., J.M. van Alphen Jacques, and F. Witte. 1997. Cichlid fish diversity threatened by eutrophication that curbs sexual selection. Science 277:1808-1811.


Female choice60 l.jpg
Female Choice parents, even when it was reared in another’s nest - suggests that resistance is at least partly genetic


Slide62 l.jpg

Female Mate Choice in Plants parents, even when it was reared in another’s nest - suggests that resistance is at least partly genetic

Waser, N.M., M.V. Price, A.M. Montalvo, and R.N. Gray. 1987. Female mate choice in a perennial herbaceous wildflower, Delphinium nelsonii. Evolutionary Trends in Plants 1:29-33.


Slide63 l.jpg

Waser et al. (1987) parents, even when it was reared in another’s nest - suggests that resistance is at least partly genetic

  • The success of a cross between two plants should depend in part on their genetic similarity - i.e., an optimal separation or optimal outcrossing distance; an index of genetic similarity

    • the optimal outcrossing distance for Delphinium nelsonii is 3-10m



Slide65 l.jpg

Waser et al. (1987) survive best under field conditions

  • These effects of outcrossing distance on fitness components are mirrored in the performance of male gametophytes

  • In a series of controlled crosses, pollen donors 10m from the recipient had a significantly higher probability of delivering a pollen tube to the ovary than did pollen from donors 1m or 100m away


Slide66 l.jpg

Waser et al. (1987) survive best under field conditions


Slide67 l.jpg

Waser et al. (1987) survive best under field conditions

  • This suggests a physiological interaction between pollen and pistil that increases the probability of producing offspring with high viability

  • Such interaction can be interpreted as adaptive discrimination or choice of mates on the part of the female


ad